Loading...
|
Please use this identifier to cite or link to this item:
https://nccur.lib.nccu.edu.tw/handle/140.119/76398
|
Title: | 台灣華語送氣與非送氣子音之對比:以語音實驗為例證 The status of aspirated and unaspirated consonants in Mandarin: Evidence from phonological experiments |
Authors: | 李冠霆 Lee, Kuan Ting |
Contributors: | 萬依萍 Wan, I Ping 李冠霆 Lee, Kuan Ting |
Keywords: | 送氣/非送氣子音 嗓音起始時間 語誤 方向性 標記 Aspirated/ unaspirated consonants voice onset time speech errors directionality markedness |
Date: | 2015 |
Issue Date: | 2015-07-13 11:03:33 (UTC+8) |
Abstract: | 送氣與非送氣的對比是台灣華語塞音及塞擦音之特性。為討論送氣的議題,本篇論文透過語音實驗,從兩方面來探討:其一為嗓音起始時間(voice onset time),另一則為語誤的方向性(directionality)及標記 (markedness)。 本研究有兩個實驗,皆以四字非詞為實驗材料。實驗一為語誤實驗,此實驗結合繞口令以及立即回想的方法來誘發語誤;實驗二則為嗓音起始時間實驗,只讓受試者回想,並未結合繞口令。這兩個實驗的設計由於受到中文本身音韻限制及詞彙空缺的限制,韻母使用了單母音(V)及韻母(VG或GV)兩種形式,因此實驗二亦將母音對子音數值的影響之比較結果納入討論。 嗓音起始時間實驗的結果顯示,使用四字非詞並結合回想作為實驗內容所測量出的數值與前人使用實詞所測量出的數值並無太大不同,並支持送氣子音的數值較非送氣子音的大。此外,塞音的研究結果支持Cho & Ladefoged (1999)所提出的原則,也就是塞音除阻的位置若越後面,則其數值會越大;若將塞音的數值套用Cho & Ladefoged (1999)的分類,則中文的非送氣塞音確為非送氣子音,而送氣塞音的數值則介於稍微送氣及送氣子音之間。塞擦音的研究結果若如Lai (2013)般套用Cho & Ladefoged (1999)之原則,則會發現送氣塞擦音符合此原則,但非送氣塞擦音卻非如此。另外,子音後接的母音或韻母亦會影響子音之嗓音起始時間。研究結果顯示當大部份子音後接單母音時,子音之嗓音起始時間較長;研究結果亦指出當子音後接的為單母音[a]或以[a]開頭的韻母時,子音的嗓音起始時間會較接其他母音如[u], [i], [ow]時短。 語誤實驗的結果顯示,除了[k], [kh]有以有標送氣塞音取代無標非送氣塞音的情形外,其他組塞音及所有塞擦音皆無以有標送氣子音取代無標非送氣子音(反之亦然)的情形。就方向性而言,研究結果顯示語誤來源出現在語誤之前(perseveration)的方向性顯著最多。 The contrast of aspiration is one of the characteristics of stops and affricates in Taiwan Mandarin. Through the conduction of two phonological experiments, we discuss the issue of aspiration from two aspects: one is from voice onset time (VOT), and the other one from directionality and markedness of speech errors. Non-word quadruple sets were used in both two experiments. The first experiment was speech errors, which combined tongue twister and immediate recall to induce speech errors; the second experiment was voice onset time, which only had the subjects to recall. Due to the limitation of phonological ill-form and lexical gaps, the rhymes of the two experiments had two forms, V and VG/GV; as a result, how vowels affected the VOT of stops and affricates were also discussed. The findings of the second experiment (voice onset time) showed that the VOT measured by using non-word quadruple set was similar to the VOT measured by using real words as stimuli, and the findings also indicated that the VOT of the aspirated consonants was longer than that of the unaspirated ones. In terms of the results of the stops, they supported the principle proposed by Cho & Ladefoged (1999) that the duration of VOT has a further back relationship with the closure; furthermore, the categories proposed by Cho & Ladefoged (1999) were applied onto the results, it showed that Mandarin unaspirated stops were indeed unaspirated consonants, and the aspirated stops fell between slightly aspirated and aspirated consonants. The findings of the aspirated stops, if applied the principle proposed by Cho & Ladefoged (1999) like Lai (2013), revealed that aspirated affricates supported the principle, yet the unaspirated ones did not. Regarding the influence of vowels on the VOT, it was found that when the succeeding vowel was a single vowel [a] or rhymes beginning with [a], the VOT of the stops and the affricates would be shorter. The findings of the first experiment (speech errors) indicated that with regard to the stops, the aspirated velar stop tended to replace the unaspirated counterpart, while for the other stops, the probability for marked aspirated stops to replace unmarked unaspirated ones or vice versa was of the same. With regard to the directionality of speech errors, the findings showed that perseverations significantly outnumbered anticipations and exchange. |
Reference: | 吳宗濟(1987)。普通話輔音不送氣/送氣區別的實驗研究。中國語言學報,3,256-283。 鄭靜宜(2011)。語音聲學—說話聲音的科學。台北:心理出版社。 Acheson, D. J., & MacDonald, M. C. (2009). Twisting tongues and memories: Explorations of the relationship between language production and verbal working memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 60, 329-350. Baars, B. J., Motley, M. T., & MacKay, D. G. (1975). Output editing for lexical status in artificially elicited slips of the tongue. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14, 382-391. Baars, B. J., & MacKay, D. G. (1978). Experimentally eliciting phonetic and sentential speech errors: methods, implications, and work in progress. Language in Society 7(1), 105-109. Baars, B. J. (1980). On eliciting predictable speech errors in the laboratory. In V. A. Fromkin (Ed.), Errors in Linguistic Performance: Slips of the Tongue, Ear, Pen, and Hand (pp. 307-318). New York, NY: Academic Press. Baars, B. J. (1992). A dozen Competing-Plans techniques for inducing predictable slips in speech and action. In B. J. Baars (Ed.), Experimental Slips and Human Error (pp. 129-150). New York, NY: Plenum Press. Berg, T. (1987). A cross-linguistic comparison of slips of the tongue. Bloomington: Indiana University Linguistics Club. Blumstein, S. E. (1973). A phonological investigation of aphasic speech. The Hague: Mouton. Chen, K., Tsay, J., & Hong, G. (1998). Duration of initials in Mandarin: Fundamental acoustic research and its clinical significance. Journal of Speech Langauge-hearing Association, 13, 138-149. Chen, L-M., Chao, K-Y., & Peng, J-F. (2007). VOT productions of word-initial stops in Mandarin and English: A cross-language study. Proceedings of the 19th Conference on Computational Linguistics and Speech Processing, 303-317. Cho, T., & Ladefoged, P. (1999). Variation and universals in VOT: Evidence from 18 languages. Journal of Phonetics, 27, 207-229. Ellis, A. W. (1980). Errors in speech and short-term memory: The effects of phonemic similarity and syllable position. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 624-634. Fellbaum, M. L. (1983). Markedness and allophonic rules. In F. R. Eckman, E. A. Moravcisik & J. R. Wirth (Eds.), Markedness (pp. 291-308). New York, NY: Plenum Press. Fromkin, V. A. (1971). The non-anomalous nature of anomalous utterances. Language, 47(1), 27-52. Fromkin, V. A. (1973). The non-anomalous nature of anomalous utterances. In V. A. Fromkin (Ed.), Speech errors as linguistic evidence (pp. 215-242). The Hague: Mouton. Fromkin, V. A., Rodman, R., & Hyams, N. (2007). An Introduction to Language. 8th Edition. Boston, MA: Wadsworth Cengage Learning. Gandour, J. (1977). Counterfeit tones in the speech of southern Thai bidilectals. Lingua, 41, 125–143. Goldrick, M., & Blumstein, S. E. (2006). Cascading activation from phonological planning to articulatory processes: Evidence from tongue twisters. Language and Cognitive Processes, 21(6), 649-683. Gormley, A. (2014). Acoustic evidence for phonologically mismatched speech errors. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 1-13. Howell, P., & Rosen, S. (1983). Production and perception of rise time in the voiceless affricate/fricative distinction. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 73, 976-984. Hsu, H-L. (2011). The emergence of an unmarkedness effect in Mandarin speech errors: Nasals in a coda position. Language and Speech, 54(3), 307-340. Hume, E. (2011). Markedness. In M. Van Oostendorp, C. Ewen, E. Hume & K. Rice (Eds), The Blackwell Companion to Phonology (pp.79-106). Blackwell. Jaeger, J. (2004). Kids’ slips: What young children’s slips of the tongue reveal about language development. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum. Jeng, J-Y. (2005). The influence of speaking rate, utterance unit and position on segmental duration of Mandarin. Journal of National University of Tainan, 39(1), 161-185. Johnson, K. (2002) Acoustic and Auditory Phonetics. 2nd Edition. Oxford: Blackwell. Keating, P. A. (1984). Phonetic and phonological representation of stop consonant voicing. Language, 60(2), 286-319. Kent, R. D., & Read, C. (2002). The acoustic analysis of speech. San Diego, CA: Singular Publishing. Ladefoged, P., & Johnson, K. (2011). A course in phonetics. Boston, MA: Cengage Learning. Lai, Y-H. (2013). VOT for Mandarin stops and affricates produced by L1 and L2 speakers of Mandarin Chinese. Kaohsiung Normal University Journal, 35, 57-78. Lin, Y. H. (2007). The Sounds of Chinese. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. Lisker, L., & Abramson, A. S. (1964). A cross-language study of voicing in initial stops: Acoustical measurement. Word, 20, 384-422. Liu, H., Ng, M. L., Wan, M., Wang, S., & Zhang, Y. (2007). Effects of place of articulation and aspiration on voice onset time in Mandarin esophageal speech. Folia Phoniatr Logop, 59, 147-154. Liu, H-M., Tseng, C-H., & Tsao, F-M. (1999). The speech acoustic characteristic of Mandarin Phonetic Contrasts. Proceedings of the National Science Council (Part C: Humanities and Social Science), 9(4), 726-738. Marquardt, T. P., Reinhart, J. B., & Peterson, H. A. (1979). Markedness analysis of phonemic substitution errors in apraxia of speech. Journal of communication disorders, 12(6), 481-494. Motley, M. T., & Baars, B. J. (1976). Laboratory induction of verbal slips: A new method for psycholinguistic research. Communication Quarterly, 24(2), 28-34. Nooteboom, S. G. (1973). The tongue slips into patterns. In V. A. Fromkin (Ed.), Speech errors as linguistic evidence (pp. 144-156). The Hague: Mouton. Page, M. P. A., Madge, A., Cumming, N., & Norris, D. G. (2007). Speech errors and the phonological similarity effect in short-term memory: Evidence suggesting a common locus. Journal of Memory and Language, 56, 49-64. Peng, J-F., Chen, L-M., & Lin, Y-Y. (2009). Tonal effects on voice onset time: Stops in Mandarin and Hakka. Proceedings of the 21st Conference on Computational Linguistics and Speech Processing (ROCLING XXI), 115-123. Shattuck-Hufnagel, S., & Klatt, D. H. (1979). The limited use of distinctive features and markedness in speech production: evidence from speech error data. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18, 41-55. Shattuck-Hufnagel, S. (1992). The role of word structure in segmental serial ordering. Cognition, 42, 213-259. Stemberger, J. P. (1992). The reliability and replicability of naturalistic speech error data: A comparison with experimentally induced errors. In B. J. Baars (Ed.), Experimental Slips and Human Error (pp. 195-215). New York, NY: Plenum Press. Tse, J. K-P. (1988). A spectrographic analysis of the coronal affricates and fricatives in Mandarin Chinese. Studies in English Literature and Linguistics, 14, 149-199. Wan, I. P. (2002). Asymmetry in Mandarin consonant articulations: Evidence from slips of the tongue. Concentric: Studies in English Literature and Linguistics, 28(2), 1-25. Wan, I. P. (2007). On the phonological organization of Mandarin tones. Lingua, 117, 1715-1738. Wilshire, C. E. (1999). The “tongue twister” paradigm as a technique for studying phonological encoding. Language and Speech, 42(1), 57-82. Wolk, L. (1986). Markedness analysis of consonant error productions in apraxia of speech. Journal of communication disorders, 19(2), 133-160. Wu, H-L. (2009). Stops and affricates in Mandarin Chinese and Hakka—A VOT Analysis. MA thesis, Kaohsiung Normal University. Yiu, H. Y. (2008). Cantonese affricates: an acoustic analysis. BA thesis. The University of Hong Kong. |
Description: | 碩士 國立政治大學 語言學研究所 101555008 103 |
Source URI: | http://thesis.lib.nccu.edu.tw/record/#G0101555008 |
Data Type: | thesis |
Appears in Collections: | [語言學研究所] 學位論文
|
Files in This Item:
File |
Size | Format | |
500801.pdf | 15105Kb | Adobe PDF2 | 522 | View/Open |
|
All items in 政大典藏 are protected by copyright, with all rights reserved.
|