Reference: | 顏乃欣、廖瑞銘、楊建銘、黃淑麗、蔡介立. (2013)。台灣地區華人情緒與相關心理生理資料庫─台灣情緒圖片系統。中華心理學刊,55(4),477-492。 Anderson, A. K. (2005). Affective influences on the attentional dynamics supporting awareness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134(2), 258. Angrilli, A., Cherubini, P., Pavese, A., & Manfredini, S. (1997). The influence of affective factors on time perception. Perception & psychophysics, 59(6), 972-982. Bar-Haim, Y., Kerem, A., Lamy, D., & Zakay, D. (2010). When time slows down: The influence of threat on time perception in anxiety. Cognition and Emotion, 24(2), 255-263. Belke, B., Leder, H., & Carbon, C. C. (2015). When challenging art gets liked: Evidences for a dual preference formation process for fluent and non-fluent portraits. PloS one, 10(8), e0131796. Berlyne, D. E. (1970). Novelty, complexity, and hedonic value. Perception & psychophysics, 8(5), 279-286. Benedek, M., & Kaernbach, C. (2010). A continuous measure of phasic electrodermal activity. Journal of neuroscience methods, 190(1), 80-91. Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1994). Measuring emotion: the self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential. Journal of behavior therapy and experimental psychiatry, 25(1), 49-59. Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1999). International affective digitized sounds (IADS): Stimuli, instruction manual and affective ratings (Tech. Rep. No. B-2). Gainesville, FL: The Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida. Brielmann, A. A., & Pelli, D. (2019). Intense beauty requires intense pleasure. Frontiers in psychology, 10, 2420. Brunner, D., Kacelnik, A., & Gibbon, J. (1992). Optimal foraging and timing processes in the starling, Sturnus vulgaris: Effect of inter-capture interval. Animal Behaviour, 44(4), 597-613. Cheng, R.-K., Ali, Y. M., & Meck, W. H. (2007). Ketamine “unlocks” the reduced clock-speed effects of cocaine following extended training: evidence for dopamine–glutamate interactions in timing and time perception. Neurobiology of learning and memory, 88(2), 149-159. Church, R. M., Meck, W. H., & Gibbon, J. (1994). Application of scalar timing theory to individual trials. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 20(2), 135. Droit-Volet, S., & Gil, S. (2016). The emotional body and time perception. Cognition and Emotion, 30(4), 687-699. Droit-Volet, S., Mermillod, M., Cocenas-Silva, R., & Gil, S. (2010). The effect of expectancy of a threatening event on time perception in human adults. Emotion, 10(6), 908. Droit-Volet, S., & Wearden, J. (2002). Speeding up an internal clock in children? Effects of visual flicker on subjective duration. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section B, 55(3), 193-211. Droit‐Volet, S., Brunot, S., & Niedenthal, P. (2004). BRIEF REPORT Perception of the duration of emotional events. Cognition and Emotion, 18(6), 849-858. Fayolle, S., Gil, S., & Droit-Volet, S. (2015). Fear and time: fear speeds up the internal clock. Behavioural processes, 120, 135-140. Forster, M., Leder, H., & Ansorge, U. (2016). Exploring the subjective feeling of fluency. Experimental Psychology. Folta-Schoofs, K., Wolf, O. T., Treue, S., & Schoofs, D. (2014). Perceptual complexity, rather than valence or arousal accounts for distracter-induced overproductions of temporal durations. Acta Psychologica, 147, 51-59 Gerger, G., Pelowski, M., & Leder, H. (2018). Empathy, Einfühlung, and aesthetic experience: The effect of emotion contagion on appreciation of representational and abstract art using fEMG and SCR. Cognitive Processing, 19(2), 147-165. Gibbon, J. (1977). Scalar expectancy theory and Weber`s law in animal timing. 84(3), 279. Psychological review, 84(3), 279. Gibbon, J. (1991). Origins of scalar timing. Learning and motivation. Gil, S., & Droit-Volet, S. (2012). Emotional time distortions: the fundamental role of arousal. Cognition & emotion, 26(5), 847-862. Graf, L. K., & Landwehr, J. R. (2015). A dual-process perspective on fluency-based aesthetics: The pleasure-interest model of aesthetic liking. Personality and social psychology review, 19(4), 395-410. Graf, L. K., & Landwehr, J. R. (2017). Aesthetic pleasure versus aesthetic interest: The two routes to aesthetic liking. Frontiers in psychology, 8, 15. Graf, L. K., Mayer, S., & Landwehr, J. R. (2018). Measuring processing fluency: One versus five items. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 28(3), 393-411. Grommet, E. K., Droit-Volet, S., Gil, S., Hemmes, N. S., Baker, A. H., & Brown, B. L. (2011). Time estimation of fear cues in human observers. Behavioural processes, 86(1), 88-93. Grondin, S., Laflamme, V., & Gontier, É. (2014). Effect on perceived duration and sensitivity to time when observing disgusted faces and disgusting mutilation pictures. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 76(6), 1522-1534. Kliegl, K. M., Watrin, L., & Huckauf, A. (2015). Duration perception of emotional stimuli: Using evaluative conditioning to avoid sensory confounds. Cognition and Emotion, 29(8), 1350-1367. Kopec, C. D., & Brody, C. D. (2010). Human performance on the temporal bisection task. Brain and cognition, 74(3), 262-272. Krupinski, E., & Locher, P. (1988). Skin conductance and aesthetic evaluative responses to nonrepresentational works of art varying in symmetry. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 26(4), 355-358. Kuchinke, L., Trapp, S., Jacobs, A. M., & Leder, H. (2009). Pupillary responses in art appreciation: Effects of aesthetic emotions. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 3(3), 156. Lake, J. I., LaBar, K. S., & Meck, W. H. (2016). Emotional modulation of interval timing and time perception. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 64, 403-420. Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1997). International affective picture system (IAPS): Technical manual and affective ratings. NIMH Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention, 1, 39-58. Lee, K.-H., Seelam, K., & O`Brien, T. (2011). The relativity of time perception produced by facial emotion stimuli. Cognition & emotion, 25(8), 1471-1480. Lejeune, H. (1998). Switching or gating? The attentional challenge in cognitive models of psychological time. Behavioural processes, 44(2), 127-145. Li, L., & Tian, Y. (2020). Aesthetic preference and time: preferred painting dilates time perception. Sage Open, 10(3), 2158244020939905. Lindell, A. K., & Lindell, K. L. (2014). Beauty captures the attention of the beholder. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 26(7), 768-780. Lui, M. A., Penney, T. B., & Schirmer, A. (2011). Emotion effects on timing: attention versus pacemaker accounts. PloS one, 6(7). Marin, M. M., & Leder, H. (2018). Exploring aesthetic experiences of females: affect-related traits predict complexity and arousal responses to music and affective pictures. Personality and individual Differences, 125, 80-90. Mella, N., Conty, L., & Pouthas, V. (2011). The role of physiological arousal in time perception: psychophysiological evidence from an emotion regulation paradigm. Brain and cognition, 75(2), 182-187. Menninghaus, W., Wagner, V., Wassiliwizky, E., Schindler, I., Hanich, J., Jacobsen, T., & Koelsch, S. (2019). What are aesthetic emotions?. Psychological review , 126(2), 171. Mitrovic, A., Hegelmaier, L. M., Leder, H., & Pelowski, M. J. A. P. (2020). Does beauty capture the eye, even if it`s not (overtly) adaptive? A comparative eye-tracking study of spontaneous attention and visual preference with VAST abstract art. Acta Psychologica, 209, 103133. Ochsner, K. N., & Gross, J. J. (2005). The cognitive control of emotion. Trends in cognitive sciences, 9(5), 242-249. Penney, T. B., & Cheng, X. (2018). Duration bisection: a user’s guide. In Timing and time perception: Procedures, measures, & applications (pp. 98-127). Brill. Reber, R., Schwarz, N., & Winkielman, P. (2004). Processing fluency and aesthetic pleasure: Is beauty in the perceiver`s processing experience?. Personality and social psychology review, 8(4), 364-382. Seifried, T., & Ulrich, R. (2011). Exogenous visual attention prolongs perceived duration. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 73(1), 68-85. Steciuch, C. C., Kopatich, R. D., Feller, D. P., Durik, A. M., & Millis, K. (2019). Don`t go with your gut: Exploring the role of motivation in aesthetic experiences. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts. Tipples, J. (2011). When time stands still: fear-specific modulation of temporal bias due to threat. Emotion, 11(1), 74. Treisman, M. (1963). Temporal discrimination and the indifference interval: Implications for a model of the" internal clock". Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 77(13), 1. Tschacher, W., Greenwood, S., Kirchberg, V., Wintzerith, S., van den Berg, K., & Tröndle, M. (2012). Physiological correlates of aesthetic perception of artworks in a museum. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 6(1), 96. Wearden. (1991). Do humans possess an internal clock with scalar timing properties?. Learning and Motivation, 22(1-2), 59-83. Wearden, J. H., & Penton-Voak, I. (1995). Feeling the heat: Body temperature and the rate of subjective time, revisited. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section B, 48(2b), 129-141. Willis, J., & Todorov, A. J. P. s. (2006). First impressions: Making up your mind after a 100-ms exposure to a face. Psychological science, 17(7), 592-598. Winkielman, P., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2001). Mind at ease puts a smile on the face: psychophysiological evidence that processing facilitation elicits positive affect. Journal of personality and social psychology, 81(6), 989. Yiend, J. (2010). The effects of emotion on attention: A review of attentional processing of emotional information. Cognition and emotion, 24(1), 3-47. Zakay, D., & Block, R. A. (1997). Temporal cognition. Current directions in psychological science, 6(1), 12-16. |