English  |  正體中文  |  简体中文  |  Post-Print筆數 : 27 |  Items with full text/Total items : 113656/144643 (79%)
Visitors : 51735321      Online Users : 646
RC Version 6.0 © Powered By DSPACE, MIT. Enhanced by NTU Library IR team.
Scope Tips:
  • please add "double quotation mark" for query phrases to get precise results
  • please goto advance search for comprehansive author search
  • Adv. Search
    HomeLoginUploadHelpAboutAdminister Goto mobile version
    政大機構典藏 > 理學院 > 心理學系 > 學位論文 >  Item 140.119/59710
    Please use this identifier to cite or link to this item: https://nccur.lib.nccu.edu.tw/handle/140.119/59710


    Title: 以事件關聯電位(ERP)探索睡眠對於配對學習的促進效果
    Event-related potential (ERP) evidence of sleep facilitating effect on paired-associates learning
    Authors: 林俊成
    Lin, Chun Cheng
    Contributors: 楊建銘
    Yang, Chien Ming
    林俊成
    Lin, Chun Cheng
    Keywords: 睡眠
    慢波睡眠
    睡眠剝奪
    學習
    記憶
    記憶固化
    陳述性記憶
    記憶再認
    配對學習
    新聯結
    事件關聯電位
    N400
    sleep
    slow wave sleep
    sleep deprivation
    learning
    memory
    consolidation
    declarative memory
    new association
    recognition memory
    paired-associates learning
    event-related potential
    ERP
    N400
    Date: 2010
    Issue Date: 2013-09-03 13:29:05 (UTC+8)
    Abstract: 睡眠是否能鞏固陳述性記憶目前尚無定論。過去研究一致較支持睡眠能增進相關字詞配對的學習,但睡眠是否能增進無關字詞配對的學習,目前仍存在不一致的發現。造成該差異的原因可能是:過去研究多採用的行為測量指標,或許無法充分反映出睡眠促進記憶新聯結(new association)產生的效果。事件關聯電位(Event-related potential, ERP)的N400反映出語意記憶系統內每個字詞彼此的相關程度,因此本研究使用N400來探討睡眠強化無關字詞配對形成新聯結的電生理歷程。30名健康受試者(15位男性與15位女性,平均年齡為20.7歲) 隨機分派至睡眠組或清醒組,第一晚在學習80組無關字詞配對後,接受第一次再認記憶測驗,同時進行ERP的記錄。隨後睡眠組接受睡眠記錄(PSG),清醒組則接受整晚的睡眠剝奪,兩組受試者皆在第二晚給予8小時的躺床時間,使他們有機會充足睡眠以恢復精神,於第三天早上接受第二次再認記憶測驗及ERP記錄。在記憶測驗時,無關字詞配對分別組成促發字(prime)與目標字(target)先後出現,受試者需判斷先後出現的字詞是否為先前學過的完整配對,在測試階段同時記錄腦電波訊號。行為測量結果顯示睡眠過後,再認表現的正確率顯著提高且反應時間明顯縮短,但在睡眠剝奪後則顯示相反的結果。電生理測量發現睡眠組的N400振幅在睡眠過後較清醒組明顯降低。另外,睡眠組較清醒組有較高的正確率與較短的反應時間。睡眠組再認測驗的進步量與慢波睡眠呈現負相關,而慢波睡眠與第一次再認測驗的正確率呈現正相關,根據二階段睡眠記憶鞏固理論,慢波睡眠涉及重新組織記憶的歷程(系統性固化),因此學習表現較佳的受試者出現較多的深度睡眠,可能反應其經歷系統性固化。本研究結果顯示睡眠對於產生新聯結有明顯的增強效果,而且慢波睡眠可能參與了記憶表徵重新分配的歷程。
    The effect of sleep on declarative memory remains contradictory. Prior studies show that sleep benefits the learning of related word pairs consistently, while the learning of unrelated word pairs, however, show mixed results. It is possible that the behavioral measures used in previous studies are not sensitive enough to reveal subtle effects of sleep on new associations. N400, an event-related potential (ERP) component reflecting relatedness among words in semantic memory, was used in the present study to investigate the effect of sleep on the physiological process underlying new associations of unrelated word pairs. Participants were randomly assigned to either a Sleep group or a Wakefulness group. In the learning phase, participants were asked to memorize 80 visually presented unrelated word-pairs, followed by a pre-test phase with a recognition task. The participants then underwent either a night of nocturnal sleep (Sleep group) or sleep deprivation (Wakefulness group). A post-test was conducted after subjects had one night of recovery sleep. During both pre-test and post-test sessions, prime and target words were presented successively for the subjects to judge whether they were among the original pairs or new pairs. ERPs were recorded during both test phases. The behavioral data show that differences in improvement of recognition and decreases in reaction time from pre-test to post-test are significant between Sleep and Wakefulness groups. N400 peak amplitude attenuated significantly after sleep but not after wakefulness. The improvement of recognition negatively correlates with slow wave sleep (SWS). The number of word-pairs acquired in the learning phase, however, correlates positively with SWS. According to the two-stage memory consolidation theory of sleep, SWS involves in redistribution of memory (systematic consolidation). Therefore, that the participants with high performance showed more SWS may reflect the process of systematic consolidation. These results suggest that the sleep has an enhancing effect on the formation of novel association, and SWS may be involved in the process of redistributing memory representations.
    Reference: Agnew, H. W., Jr., Webb, W. B., & Williams, R. L. (1966). The first night effect: an EEG study of sleep. Psychophysiology, 2(3), 263-266.
    Anderson, J. E., & Holcomb, P. J. (1995). Auditory and visual semantic priming using different stimulus onset asynchronies: an event-related brain potential study. Psychophysiology, 32(2), 177-190.
    Ashcraft, M. H. (1994). Semantic Long-term memory. In M. H. Ashcraft (Ed.), Human Memory and Cognition (pp. 253-307). New York: Harpercollins College.
    Aston-Jones, G., & Bloom, F. E. (1981). Activity of norepinephrine-containing locus coeruleus neurons in behaving rats anticipates fluctuations in the sleep-waking cycle. Journal of Neuroscience, 1(8), 876-886.
    Atienza, M., Cantero, J. L., & Dominguez-Marin, E. (2002). Mismatch negativity (MMN): an objective measure of sensory memory and long-lasting memories during sleep. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 46(3), 215-225.
    Atienza, M., Cantero, J. L., & Stickgold, R. (2004). Posttraining sleep enhances automaticity in perceptual discrimination. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(1), 53-64.
    Aubrey, J., Smith, C., Tweed, S., & Nader, R. (1999). Cognitive and motor procedural tasks are dissociated in REM and Stage 2 sleep. Sleep Research Online, 2(Supplement 1), 220.
    Backhaus, J., & Junghanns, K. (2006). Daytime naps improve procedural motor memory. Sleep Medicine, 7(6), 508-512.
    Barrett, T. R., & Ekstrand, B. R. (1972). Effect of sleep on memory. 3. Controlling for time-of-day effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 96(2), 321-327.
    Benson, K., & Feinberg, I. (1977). The beneficial effect of sleep in an extended Jenkins and Dallenbach paradigm. Psychophysiology, 14(4), 375-384.
    Bentin, S., McCarthy, G., & Wood, C. C. (1985). Event-related potentials, lexical decision and semantic priming. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 60(4), 343-355.
    Besson, M., Kutas, M., & Van Petten, C. (1992). An event-related potential (ERP) analysis of semantic congruity and repetition effects in sentences. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 4(2), 132-149.
    Born, J. (2010). Slow-wave sleep and the consolidation of long-term memory. The World Journal of Biological Psychiatry 11 Suppl 1, 16-21.
    Born, J., & Wilhelm, I. (2011). System consolidation of memory during sleep. Psychological Research. Advance online publication. doi: 10.1007/s00426-011-0335-6
    Brualla, J., Romero, M. F., Serrano, M., & Valdizan, J. R. (1998). Auditory event-related potentials to semantic priming during sleep. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 108(3), 283-290.
    Buzsaki, G. (1989). Two-stage model of memory trace formation: a role for "noisy" brain states. Neuroscience, 31(3), 551-570.
    Canas, J. J. (1990). Associative strength effects in the lexical decision task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. A, Human Experimental psychology, 42(1), 121-145.
    Castaldo, V., Krynicki, V., & Goldstein, J. (1974). Sleep stages and verbal memory. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 39, 1023-1030.
    Chernik, D. A. (1972). Effect of REM sleep deprivation on learning and recall by humans. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 34(1), 283-294.
    Chinese Knowledge and Information Processing Group (1993). The CKIP Categorical Classification of Mandarin Chinese (In Chinese). CKIP Technical Report no. 93-05.
    Chwilla, D. J., Brown, C. M., & Hagoort, P. (1995). The N400 as a function of the level of processing. Psychophysiology, 32(3), 274-285.
    Chwilla, D. J., & Kolk, H. H. (2005). Accessing world knowledge: evidence from N400 and reaction time priming. Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research, 25(3), 589-606.
    Clemens, Z., Fabo, D., & Halasz, P. (2005). Overnight verbal memory retention correlates with the number of sleep spindles. Neuroscience, 132(2), 529-535.
    Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82(6), 407-428.
    De Koninck, J., Lorrain, D., Christ, G., Proulx, G., & Coulombe, D. (1989). Intensive language learning and increases in rapid eye movement sleep: evidence of a performance factor. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 8(1), 43-47.
    Deacon, D., Dynowska, A., Ritter, W., & Grose-Fifer, J. (2004). Repetition and semantic priming of nonwords: implications for theories of N400 and word recognition. Psychophysiology, 41(1), 60-74.
    Deacon, D., Hewitt, S., Yang, C., & Nagata, M. (2000). Event-related potential indices of semantic priming using masked and unmasked words: evidence that the N400 does not reflect a post-lexical process. Brain Research: Cognitive Brain Research, 9(2), 137-146.
    Deacon, D., Uhm, T. J., Ritter, W., Hewitt, S., & Dynowska, A. (1999). The lifetime of automatic semantic priming effects may exceed two seconds. Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research, 7(4), 465-472.
    Diekelmann, S., & Born, J. (2010). The memory function of sleep. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience.
    Diekelmann, S., Wilhelm, I., & Born, J. (2009). The whats and whens of sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 13(5), 309-321.
    Doyon, J., Penhune, V., & Ungerleider, L. G. (2003). Distinct contribution of the cortico-striatal and cortico-cerebellar systems to motor skill learning. Neuropsychologia, 41(3), 252-262.
    Eichenbaum, H. (2000). A cortical-hippocampal system for declarative memory. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 1(1), 41-50.
    Ekstrand, B. R., Sullivan, M. J., Parker, D. F., & West, J. N. (1971). Spontaneous recovery and sleep. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 88(1), 142-144.
    Ellenbogen, J. M., Hulbert, J. C., Stickgold, R., Dinges, D. F., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2006). Interfering with theories of sleep and memory: sleep, declarative memory, and associative interference. Current Biology, 16(13), 1290-1294.
    Ellenbogen, J. M., Payne, J. D., & Stickgold, R. (2006). The role of sleep in declarative memory consolidation: passive, permissive, active or none? Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 16(6), 716-722.
    Empson, J. A., & Clarke, P. R. (1970). Rapid eye movements and remembering. Nature, 227(5255), 287-288.
    Ficca, G., Lombardo, P., Rossi, L., & Salzarulo, P. (2000). Morning recall of verbal material depends on prior sleep organization. Behavioural Brain Research, 112(1-2), 159-163.
    Ficca, G., & Salzarulo, P. (2004). What in sleep is for memory. Sleep Medicine, 5(3), 225-230.
    Fischer, S., Hallschmid, M., Elsner, A. L., & Born, J. (2002). Sleep forms memory for finger skills. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 99(18), 11987-11991.
    Fogel, S. M., & Smith, C. T. (2006). Learning-dependent changes in sleep spindles and Stage 2 sleep. Journal of Sleep Research, 15(3), 250-255.
    Fowler, M. J., Sullivan, M. J., & Ekstrand, B. R. (1973). Sleep and memory. Science, 179(70), 302-304.
    Franklin, M. S., Dien, J., Neely, J. H., Huber, E., & Waterson, L. D. (2007). Semantic priming modulates the N400, N300, and N400RP. Clinical Neurophysiology, 118(5), 1053-1068.
    Friederici, A. D. (1997). Neurophysiological aspects of language processing. Clinical Neuroscience, 4(2), 64-72.
    Gais, S., Albouy, G., Boly, M., Dang-Vu, T. T., Darsaud, A., Desseilles, M., et al. (2007). Sleep transforms the cerebral trace of declarative memories. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104(47), 18778-18783.
    Gais, S., & Born, J. (2004). Low acetylcholine during slow-wave sleep is critical for declarative memory consolidation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 101(7), 2140-2144.
    Gais, S., Lucas, B., & Born, J. (2006). Sleep after learning aids memory recall. Learning & Memory, 13(3), 259-262.
    Gais, S., Molle, M., Helms, K., & Born, J. (2002). Learning-dependent increases in sleep spindle density. Journal of Neuroscience, 22(15), 6830-6834.
    Gais, S., Plihal, W., Wagner, U., & Born, J. (2000). Early sleep triggers memory for early visual discrimination skills. Nature Neuroscience, 3(12), 1335-1339.
    Gold, B. T., Balota, D. A., Jones, S. J., Powell, D. K., Smith, C. D., & Andersen, A. H. (2006). Dissociation of automatic and strategic lexical-semantics: functional magnetic resonance imaging evidence for differing roles of multiple frontotemporal regions. The Journal of Neuroscience, 26(24), 6523-6532.
    Goshen-Gottstein, Y., & Moscovitch, M. (1995a). Repetition priming effects for newly formed associations are perceptually based: evidence from shallow encoding and format specificity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition 21(5), 1249-1262.
    Goshen-Gottstein, Y., & Moscovitch, M. (1995b). Repetition priming for newly formed and preexisting associations: perceptual and conceptual influences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition 21(5), 1229-1248.
    Graf, P., & Schacter, D. L. (1985). Implicit and explicit memory for new associations in normal and amnesic subjects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition 11(3), 501-518.
    Grosvenor, A., & Lack, L. C. (1984). The effect of sleep before or after learning on memory. Sleep, 7(2), 155-167.
    Haider, M., Spong, P., & Lindsley, D. B. (1964). Attention, vigilance, and cortical evoked-potentials in humans. Science, 145, 180-182.
    Hebb, D. O. (1949). The orgnizaion of behavior. New York: John Wiley and Sons.


    Hill, H., Ott, F., & Weisbrod, M. (2005). SOA-dependent N400 and P300 semantic priming effects using pseudoword primes and a delayed lexical decision. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 56(3), 209-221.
    Hill, H., Strube, M., Roesch-Ely, D., & Weisbrod, M. (2002). Automatic vs. controlled processes in semantic priming--differentiation by event-related potentials. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 44(3), 197-218.
    Hobson, J. A., McCarley, R. W., & Wyzinski, P. W. (1975). Sleep cycle oscillation: reciprocal discharge by two brainstem neuronal groups. Science, 189(4196), 55-58.
    Holcomb, P. J. (1988). Automatic and attentional processing: an event-related brain potential analysis of semantic priming. Brain and Language, 35(1), 66-85.
    Holcomb, P. J., & Neville, H. J. (1990). Auditory and visual semantic priming in lexical decision: A comparison using event-related brain potentials. Language and Cognitive Processes, 5(4), 281-312.
    Jenkins, J. G., & Dallenbach, K. M. (1924). Obliviscence during sleep and waking The American Journal of Psychology, 35(4), 605-612.
    Kametani, H., & Kawamura, H. (1990). Alterations in acetylcholine release in the rat hippocampus during sleep-wakefulness detected by intracerebral dialysis. Life Sciences, 47(5), 421-426.
    Karni, A., Tanne, D., Rubenstein, B. S., Askenasy, J. J., & Sagi, D. (1994). Dependence on REM sleep of overnight improvement of a perceptual skill. Science, 265(5172), 679-682.
    Kounios, J., & Holcomb, P. J. (1992). Structure and process in semantic memory: evidence from event-related brain potentials and reaction times. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 121(4), 459-479.
    Kuperberg, G. R., Lakshmanan, B. M., Greve, D. N., & West, W. C. (2008). Task and semantic relationship influence both the polarity and localization of hemodynamic modulation during lexico-semantic processing. Human Brain Mapping, 29(5), 544-561.
    Kuriyama, K., Stickgold, R., & Walker, M. P. (2004). Sleep-dependent learning and motor-skill complexity. Learning & Memory, 11(6), 705-713.
    Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K. D. (2000). Electrophysiology reveals semantic memory use in language comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(12), 463-470.
    Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K. D. (2011). Thirty years and counting: finding meaning in the N400 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP). Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 621-647.
    Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1980). Reading senseless sentences: brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science, 207(4427), 203-205.
    Kutas, M., & Iragui, V. (1998). The N400 in a semantic categorization task across 6 decades. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 108(5), 456-471.
    Lau, E. F., Phillips, C., & Poeppel, D. (2008). A cortical network for semantics: (de)constructing the N400. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 9(12), 920-933.
    Lewin, I., & Glaubman, H. (1975). The effect of REM deprivation: is it detrimental, beneficial, or neutral? Psychophysiology, 12(3), 349-353.
    Lin, C. C., & Yang, C. M. (2010). Event-related potential indices of new word association following intensive learning of unrelated word-pairs. Poster presented at the 49th Annual Meeting of the Taiwanese Psychological Association.
    Lund, H. G., Reider, B. D., Whiting, A. B., & Prichard, J. R. (2010). Sleep patterns and predictors of disturbed sleep in a large population of college students. Journal of Adolescent Health, 46(2), 124-132.
    Lydic, R., & Baghdoyan, H. A. (1998). Handbook of Behavioral State Control: Cellular and Molecular Mechanisms: Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.
    Maquet, P. (2001). The role of sleep in learning and memory. Science, 294(5544), 1048-1052.
    Maquet, P., Laureys, S., Peigneux, P., Fuchs, S., Petiau, C., Phillips, C., et al. (2000). Experience-dependent changes in cerebral activation during human REM sleep. Nature Neuroscience, 3(8), 831-836.
    Maquet, P., Schwartz, S., Passingham, R., & Frith, C. (2003). Sleep-related consolidation of a visuomotor skill: brain mechanisms as assessed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Journal of Neuroscience, 23(4), 1432-1440.
    Marrosu, F., Portas, C., Mascia, M. S., Casu, M. A., Fa, M., Giagheddu, M., et al. (1995). Microdialysis measurement of cortical and hippocampal acetylcholine release during sleep-wake cycle in freely moving cats. Brain Research, 671(2), 329-332.
    Marshall, L., & Born, J. (2007). The contribution of sleep to hippocampus-dependent memory consolidation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(10), 442-450.
    Mazzoni, G., Gori, S., Formicola, G., Gneri, C., Massetani, R., Murri, L., et al. (1999). Word recall correlates with sleep cycles in elderly subjects. Journal of Sleep Research, 8(3), 185-188.
    McKone, E., & Slee, J. A. (1997). Explicit contamination in "implicit" memory for new associations. Memory & cognition, 25(3), 352-366.
    Meienberg, P. (1977). The tonic aspects of human REM sleep during long-term intensive verbal learning. Physiological Psychology, 5, 250-256.
    Mograss, M., Godbout, R., & Guillem, F. (2006). The ERP old-new effect: A useful indicator in studying the effects of sleep on memory retrieval processes. Sleep, 29(11), 1491-1500.
    Mograss, M., Guillem, F., Brazzini-Poisson, V., & Godbout, R. (2009). The effects of total sleep deprivation on recognition memory processes: a study of event-related potential. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 91(4), 343-352.
    Mograss, M., Guillem, F., & Godbout, R. (2008). Event-related potentials differentiates the processes involved in the effects of sleep on recognition memory. Psychophysiology.
    Näätänen, R., Gaillard, A., & Mäntysalo, S. (1978). The N1 effect of selective attention reinterpreted. Acta Psychologica, 42, 312-329.
    Neely, J. H. (1991). Semantic priming effects in visual word recognition: a selective review of current findings and theories. In D. Besner, Humphreys, G.W. (Ed.), Basic progresses in reading—visual word recognition (pp. 264-333). NJ: Erlbaum, Hillsdale.
    Nunez-Pena, M. I., & Honrubia-Serrano, M. L. (2005). N400 and category exemplar associative strength. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 56(1), 45-54.
    Pace-Schott, E. F., & Hobson, J. A. (2002). The neurobiology of sleep: genetics, cellular physiology and subcortical networks. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 3(8), 591-605.
    Peigneux, P., Laureys, S., Delbeuck, X., & Maquet, P. (2001). Sleeping brain, learning brain. The role of sleep for memory systems. Neuroreport, 12(18), A111-124.
    Peigneux, P., Laureys, S., Fuchs, S., Collette, F., Perrin, F., Reggers, J., et al. (2004). Are spatial memories strengthened in the human hippocampus during slow wave sleep? Neuron, 44(3), 535-545.
    Peters, K. R., Smith, V., & Smith, C. T. (2007). Changes in sleep architecture following motor learning depend on initial skill level. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(5), 817-829.
    Plihal, W., & Born, J. (1997). Effects of early and late nocturnal sleep on declarative and procedural memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 534-547.
    Plihal, W., & Born, J. (1999). Effects of early and late nocturnal sleep on priming and spatial memory. Psychophysiology, 36(5), 571-582.
    Rasch, B., & Born, J. (2007). Maintaining memories by reactivation. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 17(6), 698-703.
    Rasch, B., Buchel, C., Gais, S., & Born, J. (2007). Odor cues during slow-wave sleep prompt declarative memory consolidation. Science, 315(5817), 1426-1429.
    Rauchs, G., Desgranges, B., Foret, J., & Eustache, F. (2005). The relationships between memory systems and sleep stages. Journal of Sleep Research, 14(2), 123-140.
    Rechtschaffen, A., & Kales, A. (1968). A manual of standardized terminology, techniques and scoring systems for sleep stages of human subjects: Los Angeles: Brain Information Service/Brain Research Institute.
    Rhodes, S. M., & Donaldson, D. I. (2008). Association and not semantic relationships elicit the N400 effect: electrophysiological evidence from an explicit language comprehension task. Psychophysiology, 45(1), 50-59.
    Roehm, D., Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, I., Rosler, F., & Schlesewsky, M. (2007). To predict or not to predict: influences of task and strategy on the processing of semantic relations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(8), 1259-1274.
    Rossell, S. L., Price, C. J., & Nobre, A. C. (2003). The anatomy and time course of semantic priming investigated by fMRI and ERPs. Neuropsychologia, 41(5), 550-564.
    Rudoy, J. D., Voss, J. L., Westerberg, C. E., & Paller, K. A. (2009). Strengthening individual memories by reactivating them during sleep. Science, 326(5956), 1079.
    Rugg, M. D. (1985). The effects of semantic priming and work repetition on event-related potentials. Psychophysiology, 22(6), 642-647.
    Rugg, M. D. (1990). Event-related brain potentials dissociate repetition effects of high- and low-frequency words. Memory & Cognition, 18(4), 367-379.
    Rugg, M. D., & Doyle, M. C. (1994). Event-related potentials and stimulus repetition in indirect and direct. In H. Heinze, T. Munte & G. R. Mangun (Eds.), Cognitive Electrophysiology (pp. 124-148). Boston: Birkhauser.
    Schacter, D. L., & Buckner, R. L. (1998). Priming and the brain. Neuron, 20(2), 185-195.
    Schmidt, C., Peigneux, P., Muto, V., Schenkel, M., Knoblauch, V., Munch, M., et al. (2006). Encoding difficulty promotes postlearning changes in sleep spindle activity during napping. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(35), 8976-8982.
    Sejnowski, T. J., & Destexhe, A. (2000). Why do we sleep? Brain Research, 886(1-2), 208-223.
    Shima, K., Nakahama, H., & Yamamoto, M. (1986). Firing properties of two types of nucleus raphe dorsalis neurons during the sleep-waking cycle and their responses to sensory stimuli. Brain Research, 399(2), 317-326.
    Smith, C. (1995). Sleep states and memory processes. Behavioural Brain Research, 69(1-2), 137-145.
    Smith, C. (2001). Sleep states and memory processes in humans: procedural versus declarative memory systems. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 5(6), 491-506.
    Smith, C., & Lapp, L. (1991). Increases in number of REMS and REM density in humans following an intensive learning period. Sleep, 14(4), 325-330.
    Smith, C., & MacNeill, C. (1994). Impaired motor memory for a pursuit rotor task following Stage 2 sleep loss in college students. Journal of Sleep Research, 3(4), 206-213.
    Smith, C., & Weeden, K. (1990). Post training REMs coincident auditory stimulation enhances memory in humans. Psychiatric Journal of the University of Ottawa, 15(2), 85-90.
    Smith, C. T., Nixon, M. R., & Nader, R. S. (2004). Posttraining increases in REM sleep intensity implicate REM sleep in memory processing and provide a biological marker of learning potential. Learning & Memory, 11(6), 714-719.
    Squire, L. R. (1986). Mechanisms of memory. Science, 232(4758), 1612-1619.
    Squire, L. R., & Zola, S. M. (1996). Structure and function of declarative and nondeclarative memory systems. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 93(24), 13515-13522.
    Stickgold, R. (1998). Sleep: off-line memory reprocessing. Trends in Cognitive Science, 2(12), 484-492.
    Stickgold, R. (2004). Dissecting sleep-dependent learning and memory consolidation. Comment on Schabus M et al. Sleep spindles and their significance for declarative memory consolidation. Sleep 2004;27(8):1479-85. Sleep, 27(8), 1443-1445.
    Stickgold, R. (2005). Sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Nature, 437(7063), 1272-1278.
    Stickgold, R., James, L., & Hobson, J. A. (2000). Visual discrimination learning requires sleep after training. Nature Neuroscience, 3(12), 1237-1238.
    Stickgold, R., Scott, L., Rittenhouse, C., & Hobson, J. A. (1999). Sleep-induced changes in associative memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11(2), 182-193.
    Stickgold, R., Whidbee, D., Schirmer, B., Patel, V., & Hobson, J. A. (2000). Visual discrimination task improvement: A multi-step process occurring during sleep. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(2), 246-254.
    Su, J.Y. (2010). Study of Promotion of Procedural Memory Consolidation by Auditory Stimulus during Sleep (Unpublished Master`s Thesis). National Cheng Kung University, Tainan,Taiwan.
    Talamini, L. M., Nieuwenhuis, I. L., Takashima, A., & Jensen, O. (2008). Sleep directly following learning benefits consolidation of spatial associative memory. Learning & Memory, 15(4), 233-237.
    Tamminen, J., Payne, J. D., Stickgold, R., Wamsley, E. J., & Gaskell, M. G. (2010). Sleep spindle activity is associated with the integration of new memories and existing knowledge. The Journal of Neuroscience, 30(43), 14356-14360.
    Tilley, A. J., & Empson, J. A. (1978). REM sleep and memory consolidation. Biological Psychology, 6(4), 293-300.
    Tucker, M. A., Hirota, Y., Wamsley, E. J., Lau, H., Chaklader, A., & Fishbein, W. (2006). A daytime nap containing solely non-REM sleep enhances declarative but not procedural memory. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 86(2), 241-247.
    Tulving, E. (1972). Episodic and semantic memory: New York: Academic Press.
    Tulving, E., & Schacter, D. L. (1990). Priming and human memory systems. Science, 247(4940), 301-306.
    Van Petten, C., Kutas, M., Kluender, R., Mitchiner, M., & Mclsaac, H. (1991). Fractionating the word repetition effect with event-related potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 3(2), 131-150.
    Velluti, R. A. (1997). Interactions between sleep and sensory physiology. Journal of Sleep Research, 6(2), 61-77.
    Vertes, R. P. (2004). Memory consolidation in sleep; dream or reality. Neuron, 44(1), 135-148.
    Vertes, R. P., & Eastman, K. E. (2000). The case against memory consolidation in REM sleep. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23(6), 867-876; discussion 904-1121.
    Vertes, R. P., & Siegel, J. M. (2005). Time for the sleep community to take a critical look at the purported role of sleep in memory processing. Sleep, 28(10), 1228-1229; discussion 1230-1223.
    Wagner, A. D., & Koutstaal, W. (2002). Priming. In V. S. Ramachandran (Ed.), Encyclopedia of the Human Brain, Four-Volume Set (Vol. 4, pp. 27-46). New York: Academic Press.
    Walker, M. P. (2005). A refined model of sleep and the time course of memory formation. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(1), 51-64; discussion 64-104.
    Walker, M. P., Brakefield, T., Morgan, A., Hobson, J. A., & Stickgold, R. (2002). Practice with sleep makes perfect: sleep-dependent motor skill learning. Neuron, 35(1), 205-211.
    Walker, M. P., Liston, C., Hobson, J. A., & Stickgold, R. (2002). Cognitive flexibility across the sleep-wake cycle: REM-sleep enhancement of anagram problem solving. Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research, 14(3), 317-324.
    Walker, M. P., & Stickgold, R. (2006). Sleep, memory, and plasticity. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 139-166.
    Wamsley, E. J., Tucker, M., Payne, J. D., Benavides, J. A., & Stickgold, R. (2010). Dreaming of a learning task is associated with enhanced sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Current Biology 20(9), 850-855.
    Yaroush, R., Sullivan, M. J., & Ekstrand, B. R. (1971). Effect of sleep on memory. II. Differential effect of the first and second half of the night. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 88(3), 361-366.
    Young, M. P., & Rugg, M. D. (1992). Word frequency and multiple repetition as determinants of the modulation of event-related potentials in a semantic classification task. Psychophysiology, 29(6), 664-676.
    Zola, S. M. (1998). Memory, amnesia, and the issue of recovered memory: neurobiological aspects. Clinical Psychology Review, 18(8), 915-932.
    Description: 碩士
    國立政治大學
    心理學研究所
    95752002
    99
    Source URI: http://thesis.lib.nccu.edu.tw/record/#G0957520021
    Data Type: thesis
    Appears in Collections:[心理學系] 學位論文

    Files in This Item:

    File Description SizeFormat
    002101.pdf1858KbAdobe PDF2425View/Open


    All items in 政大典藏 are protected by copyright, with all rights reserved.


    社群 sharing

    著作權政策宣告 Copyright Announcement
    1.本網站之數位內容為國立政治大學所收錄之機構典藏,無償提供學術研究與公眾教育等公益性使用,惟仍請適度,合理使用本網站之內容,以尊重著作權人之權益。商業上之利用,則請先取得著作權人之授權。
    The digital content of this website is part of National Chengchi University Institutional Repository. It provides free access to academic research and public education for non-commercial use. Please utilize it in a proper and reasonable manner and respect the rights of copyright owners. For commercial use, please obtain authorization from the copyright owner in advance.

    2.本網站之製作,已盡力防止侵害著作權人之權益,如仍發現本網站之數位內容有侵害著作權人權益情事者,請權利人通知本網站維護人員(nccur@nccu.edu.tw),維護人員將立即採取移除該數位著作等補救措施。
    NCCU Institutional Repository is made to protect the interests of copyright owners. If you believe that any material on the website infringes copyright, please contact our staff(nccur@nccu.edu.tw). We will remove the work from the repository and investigate your claim.
    DSpace Software Copyright © 2002-2004  MIT &  Hewlett-Packard  /   Enhanced by   NTU Library IR team Copyright ©   - Feedback