Reference: | Reference 金瑞芝 [Chin, J.] (1990). 親子共讀圖畫書之歷程:三歲與五歲組的比較 [The process of joint picture book reading: A comparison of three- and five-year-olds]. 台北市立師範學院學報 [Journal of Taipei Municipal Teachers College], 31, 193-208. 唐榮昌 [Tang, J-C.] (1994). 個人變項及家庭環境變項對幼兒早期閱讀能力影響之研究 [A study of the impact of the individual and home-environmental variable on early reading abilities]. Unpublished thesis. Graduate Program of Special Education, National Taiwan Normal University, Taipei, Taiwan. 黃菁芳 [Huang, J. F.] (1994). 城鄉地區學前兒童親子互動與語言能力之比較研究 [A study of parent-child interaction and children’s language ability in urban and suburban areas]. Unpublished thesis. Graduate Program of Home Economics Education, National Taiwan Normal University, Taipei, Taiwan. 臺北市政府社會局 [Department of Social Welfare, Taipei City Government] (n.d.). 社會救助手冊 [Handbook of social assistance]. Retrieved December 28, 2003, from http://www.dosw.tcg.gov.tw/newbook2/index.asp Anderson-Yockel, J. & Haynes, W. O. (1994). Joint book-reading strategies in working-class African American and White mother-toddler dyads. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 37 (3), 583-593. Britto, P. R., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2001). Beyond shared book reading: Dimensions of home literacy and low-income African American preschoolers’ skills. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 92, 73-89. Brown, R. (1977). Introduction. In C. E. Snow, & C. A. Ferguson. (Ed.), Talking to children: Language input and interaction (pp. 1-27). Cambridge: Cambridge University press. Bus, A. G., Leseman, P. P M, & Keultjes, P. (2000). Joint book reading across cultures: A comparison of Surinamese-Dutch, Turkish-Dutch, and Dutch parent-child dyads. Journal of Literacy Research, 32 (1), 53-76 Bus, A. G., van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Pellegrini, A. D. (1995). Joint book reading makes for success in learning to read: A meta-analysis on intergenerational transmission of literacy. Review of Educational Research, 65 (1), 1-21. Carroll, D. W. (1999). Psychology of language (3rd ed.). Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole. Chang, C. (2000). Narrative performance across contexts and over time: Preschool Chinese children and mothers. Unpublished Dissertation. Graduate School of Education, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, U.S.A. Chen, Y., & He, A. W. (2001). Dui bu dui as a pragmatic marker: Evidence from Chinese classroom discourse. Journal of Pragmatics, 33, 1441-1465. Cross, T. G. (1977). Mothers’ speech adjustments: The contribution of selected child listener variables. In C. E. Snow, & C. A. Ferguson. (Ed.), Talking to children: Language input and interaction (pp. 151-88). Cambridge: Cambridge University press. Day, A. (1985). Good dog, Carl. New York: Simon & Schuster. De Temple, J. M. (1994). Book reading styles of low-income mothers with preschoolers and children’s later literacy skills. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Harvard Graduate School of Education, Cambridge, MA. De Temple, J. M. (2001). Parents and children reading books together. In D. K. Dickinson, & P. O. Tabors. (Ed.), Beginning literacy with language (pp. 31-51). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes. De Temple, J. M., & Beals, D. E. (1991). Family talk: Sources of support for the development of decontextualized language skills. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 6 (1), 11-19. De Temple, J. M., & Tabors, P. O. (1995). Styles of interaction during a book reading task: Implications for literacy intervention with low-income families. In K. Q. Hinchman, D. J. Leu, & C. K. Kinzer. (Ed.), Perspectives on literacy research and practice, 44th yearbook of the National Reading Conference. Chicago: National Reading Conference. Dickinson, D. K., & De Temple, J. M. (1998). Putting parents in the picture: Maternal reports of preschoolers’ literacy as a predictor of early reading. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 13 (2), 241-261. Edwards, P. A., & Garcia, G. E. (1994). The implication of Vygotskian theory for the development of home-school programs: A focus on storybook reading. In V. John-Steiner, C. P. Panofsky, & L. W. Smith. (Ed.), Sociocultural approaches to language and literacy: An interactionist perspective (pp. 243-264). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Erbaugh, M. S. (1992). The acquisition of Mandarin. In D. I. Slobin. (Ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition, Vol.3 (pp. 373-455). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Garnica, O. (1977). Some prosodic and paralinguistic features of speech to young children. In C. E. Snow, & C. A. Ferguson. (Ed.), Talking to children: Language input and interaction (pp. 63-88). Cambridge: Cambridge University press. Haden, C. A., Reese, E., & Fivush, R. (1996). Mothers’ extratextual comments during storybook reading: Stylistic differences over time and across texts. Discourse Processes, 21, 135-169. Heath, S. B. (1986). What no bedtime story means: Narrative skills at home and school. In B. Schieffelin, & E. Ochs. (Ed.), Language socialization across cultures (pp. 97-124). Cambridge: Cambridge University press. Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1991). Mother-child conversation in different social classes and communicative settings. Child Development, 62, 782-796. Kato-Otani, E. (2003). Mother-child interactions during book reading and maternal beliefs about book reading. Unpublished Dissertation. Graduate School of Education, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, U.S.A. Kemler-Nelson, D. G., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Jusczyk, P. W., & Wright-Cassidy, K. (1989). How the prosodic cues in motherese might assist language learning. Journal of Child Language, 16, 53-68. Li, C. N., & Thompson, S. A. (1997). Mandarin Chinese: A functional reference grammar. Taipei: Crane. Lieven, E. V. M. (1994). Crosslinguistic and crosscultural aspects of language addressed to children. In C. Gallaway, & B. J. Richards. (Ed.), Input and interaction in language acquisition (pp. 56-73). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. MacWhinney, B. (1991). The CHILDES project: Computational tools for analyzing talk. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Ninio, A. (1980a). Ostensive definition in vocabulary teaching. Journal of Child Language, 7, 565-573. Ninio, A. (1980b). Picture-book reading in mother-infant dyads belonging to two subgroups in Israel. Child Development, 51, 587-590. Ninio, A. (1983). Joint book reading as a multiple vocabulary acquisition device. Developmental Psychology, 19, 445-451. Ninio, A. & Bruner, J. (1978). The achievement and antecedents of labeling. Journal of Child Language, 5, 1-14. Panofsky, C. P. (1994). Developing the representational functions of language: The role of parent-child book reading. In V. John-Steiner, C. P. Panofsky, & L. W. Smith. (Ed.), Sociocultural approaches to language and literacy: An interactionist perspective (pp. 223-242). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Ratner, N. & Bruner, J. (1978). Games, social exchange and the acquisition of language. Journal of Child Language, 5, 391-401. Sachs, J. (1977). The adaptive significance of linguistic input to prelinguistic infants. In C. E. Snow, & C. A. Ferguson. (Ed.), Talking to children: Language input and interaction (pp. 51-61). Cambridge: Cambridge University press. Sachs, J. (1983). Talking about the there and then: The emergence of displaced reference in parent-child discourse. In K. E. Nelson (Ed.), Children’s language, Vol. 4 (pp. 1-28). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum. Scarborough, H. S., & Dobrich, W. (1994). On the efficacy of reading to preschoolers. Developmental Review, 14, 245-302. Snow, C. E. (1977a). Mothers’ speech research: From input to interaction. In C. E. Snow, & C. A. Ferguson. (Ed.), Talking to children: Language input and interaction (pp. 31-49). Cambridge: Cambridge University press. Snow, C. E. (1977b). The development of conversation between mothers and babies. Journal of Child Language, 4, 1-22. Snow, C. E. (1983). Literacy and language: Relationships during the preschool years. Harvard Educational Review, 53, 165-189. Snow, C. E. (1991). The theoretical basis for relationship between language and literacy in development. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 6 (1), 5-10. Snow, C. E. (1994). Beginning from baby talk: Twenty years of research on input and interaction. In C. Gallaway, & B. J. Richards. (Ed.), Input and interaction in language acquisition (pp. 3-12). Cambridge: Cambridge University press. Snow, C. E., Arlman-Rupp, A., Hassing, Y., Jobse, J., Joosten, J., & Vorster, J. (1976). Mothers’ speech in three social classes. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 5, 1-20. Snow, C. E., & Ninio, A. (1986). The contracts of literacy: What children learn from learning to read books. In W. H. Teale, & E. Sulzby (Ed.), Emergent literacy: Writing and reading (pp. 116-138). Stamford, CT: Ablex. Sokolov, J. L., & Snow, C. E. (Ed.). (1994). Handbook of research in language development using CHILDES. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Tabors, P. O., Beals, D. E., & Weizman, Z. O. (2001) “You know what oxygen is?” Learning new words at home. In D. K. Dickinson, & P. O. Tabors (Ed.), Beginning literacy with language (pp. 93-110). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes. Tomasello, M., & Farrar, J. (1986). Joint attention and early language. Child Development, 57, 1454-1463. Vygotsky, L. (1978). Mind in Society. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. Wells, G. (1985). Preschool literacy-related activities and success in school. In D. R. Olson, N. Torrance, & A. Hildyard (Ed.), Literacy, language, and learning: The nature and consequences of reading and writing (pp. 229-255). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. |