Reference: | Agnew, H. W., Jr., Webb, W. B., & Williams, R. L. (1966). The first night effect: an EEG study of sleep. Psychophysiology, 2(3), 263-266. Anderson, J. E., & Holcomb, P. J. (1995). Auditory and visual semantic priming using different stimulus onset asynchronies: an event-related brain potential study. Psychophysiology, 32(2), 177-190. Ashcraft, M. H. (1994). Semantic Long-term memory. In M. H. Ashcraft (Ed.), Human Memory and Cognition (pp. 253-307). New York: Harpercollins College. Aston-Jones, G., & Bloom, F. E. (1981). Activity of norepinephrine-containing locus coeruleus neurons in behaving rats anticipates fluctuations in the sleep-waking cycle. Journal of Neuroscience, 1(8), 876-886. Atienza, M., Cantero, J. L., & Dominguez-Marin, E. (2002). Mismatch negativity (MMN): an objective measure of sensory memory and long-lasting memories during sleep. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 46(3), 215-225. Atienza, M., Cantero, J. L., & Stickgold, R. (2004). Posttraining sleep enhances automaticity in perceptual discrimination. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(1), 53-64. Aubrey, J., Smith, C., Tweed, S., & Nader, R. (1999). Cognitive and motor procedural tasks are dissociated in REM and Stage 2 sleep. Sleep Research Online, 2(Supplement 1), 220. Backhaus, J., & Junghanns, K. (2006). Daytime naps improve procedural motor memory. Sleep Medicine, 7(6), 508-512. Barrett, T. R., & Ekstrand, B. R. (1972). Effect of sleep on memory. 3. Controlling for time-of-day effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 96(2), 321-327. Benson, K., & Feinberg, I. (1977). The beneficial effect of sleep in an extended Jenkins and Dallenbach paradigm. Psychophysiology, 14(4), 375-384. Bentin, S., McCarthy, G., & Wood, C. C. (1985). Event-related potentials, lexical decision and semantic priming. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 60(4), 343-355. Besson, M., Kutas, M., & Van Petten, C. (1992). An event-related potential (ERP) analysis of semantic congruity and repetition effects in sentences. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 4(2), 132-149. Born, J. (2010). Slow-wave sleep and the consolidation of long-term memory. The World Journal of Biological Psychiatry 11 Suppl 1, 16-21. Born, J., & Wilhelm, I. (2011). System consolidation of memory during sleep. Psychological Research. Advance online publication. doi: 10.1007/s00426-011-0335-6 Brualla, J., Romero, M. F., Serrano, M., & Valdizan, J. R. (1998). Auditory event-related potentials to semantic priming during sleep. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 108(3), 283-290. Buzsaki, G. (1989). Two-stage model of memory trace formation: a role for "noisy" brain states. Neuroscience, 31(3), 551-570. Canas, J. J. (1990). Associative strength effects in the lexical decision task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. A, Human Experimental psychology, 42(1), 121-145. Castaldo, V., Krynicki, V., & Goldstein, J. (1974). Sleep stages and verbal memory. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 39, 1023-1030. Chernik, D. A. (1972). Effect of REM sleep deprivation on learning and recall by humans. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 34(1), 283-294. Chinese Knowledge and Information Processing Group (1993). The CKIP Categorical Classification of Mandarin Chinese (In Chinese). CKIP Technical Report no. 93-05. Chwilla, D. J., Brown, C. M., & Hagoort, P. (1995). The N400 as a function of the level of processing. Psychophysiology, 32(3), 274-285. Chwilla, D. J., & Kolk, H. H. (2005). Accessing world knowledge: evidence from N400 and reaction time priming. Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research, 25(3), 589-606. Clemens, Z., Fabo, D., & Halasz, P. (2005). Overnight verbal memory retention correlates with the number of sleep spindles. Neuroscience, 132(2), 529-535. Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82(6), 407-428. De Koninck, J., Lorrain, D., Christ, G., Proulx, G., & Coulombe, D. (1989). Intensive language learning and increases in rapid eye movement sleep: evidence of a performance factor. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 8(1), 43-47. Deacon, D., Dynowska, A., Ritter, W., & Grose-Fifer, J. (2004). Repetition and semantic priming of nonwords: implications for theories of N400 and word recognition. Psychophysiology, 41(1), 60-74. Deacon, D., Hewitt, S., Yang, C., & Nagata, M. (2000). Event-related potential indices of semantic priming using masked and unmasked words: evidence that the N400 does not reflect a post-lexical process. Brain Research: Cognitive Brain Research, 9(2), 137-146. Deacon, D., Uhm, T. J., Ritter, W., Hewitt, S., & Dynowska, A. (1999). The lifetime of automatic semantic priming effects may exceed two seconds. Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research, 7(4), 465-472. Diekelmann, S., & Born, J. (2010). The memory function of sleep. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience. Diekelmann, S., Wilhelm, I., & Born, J. (2009). The whats and whens of sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 13(5), 309-321. Doyon, J., Penhune, V., & Ungerleider, L. G. (2003). Distinct contribution of the cortico-striatal and cortico-cerebellar systems to motor skill learning. Neuropsychologia, 41(3), 252-262. Eichenbaum, H. (2000). A cortical-hippocampal system for declarative memory. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 1(1), 41-50. Ekstrand, B. R., Sullivan, M. J., Parker, D. F., & West, J. N. (1971). Spontaneous recovery and sleep. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 88(1), 142-144. Ellenbogen, J. M., Hulbert, J. C., Stickgold, R., Dinges, D. F., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2006). Interfering with theories of sleep and memory: sleep, declarative memory, and associative interference. Current Biology, 16(13), 1290-1294. Ellenbogen, J. M., Payne, J. D., & Stickgold, R. (2006). The role of sleep in declarative memory consolidation: passive, permissive, active or none? Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 16(6), 716-722. Empson, J. A., & Clarke, P. R. (1970). Rapid eye movements and remembering. Nature, 227(5255), 287-288. Ficca, G., Lombardo, P., Rossi, L., & Salzarulo, P. (2000). Morning recall of verbal material depends on prior sleep organization. Behavioural Brain Research, 112(1-2), 159-163. Ficca, G., & Salzarulo, P. (2004). What in sleep is for memory. Sleep Medicine, 5(3), 225-230. Fischer, S., Hallschmid, M., Elsner, A. L., & Born, J. (2002). Sleep forms memory for finger skills. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 99(18), 11987-11991. Fogel, S. M., & Smith, C. T. (2006). Learning-dependent changes in sleep spindles and Stage 2 sleep. Journal of Sleep Research, 15(3), 250-255. Fowler, M. J., Sullivan, M. J., & Ekstrand, B. R. (1973). Sleep and memory. Science, 179(70), 302-304. Franklin, M. S., Dien, J., Neely, J. H., Huber, E., & Waterson, L. D. (2007). Semantic priming modulates the N400, N300, and N400RP. Clinical Neurophysiology, 118(5), 1053-1068. Friederici, A. D. (1997). Neurophysiological aspects of language processing. Clinical Neuroscience, 4(2), 64-72. Gais, S., Albouy, G., Boly, M., Dang-Vu, T. T., Darsaud, A., Desseilles, M., et al. (2007). Sleep transforms the cerebral trace of declarative memories. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104(47), 18778-18783. Gais, S., & Born, J. (2004). Low acetylcholine during slow-wave sleep is critical for declarative memory consolidation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 101(7), 2140-2144. Gais, S., Lucas, B., & Born, J. (2006). Sleep after learning aids memory recall. Learning & Memory, 13(3), 259-262. Gais, S., Molle, M., Helms, K., & Born, J. (2002). Learning-dependent increases in sleep spindle density. Journal of Neuroscience, 22(15), 6830-6834. Gais, S., Plihal, W., Wagner, U., & Born, J. (2000). Early sleep triggers memory for early visual discrimination skills. Nature Neuroscience, 3(12), 1335-1339. Gold, B. T., Balota, D. A., Jones, S. J., Powell, D. K., Smith, C. D., & Andersen, A. H. (2006). Dissociation of automatic and strategic lexical-semantics: functional magnetic resonance imaging evidence for differing roles of multiple frontotemporal regions. The Journal of Neuroscience, 26(24), 6523-6532. Goshen-Gottstein, Y., & Moscovitch, M. (1995a). Repetition priming effects for newly formed associations are perceptually based: evidence from shallow encoding and format specificity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition 21(5), 1249-1262. Goshen-Gottstein, Y., & Moscovitch, M. (1995b). Repetition priming for newly formed and preexisting associations: perceptual and conceptual influences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition 21(5), 1229-1248. Graf, P., & Schacter, D. L. (1985). Implicit and explicit memory for new associations in normal and amnesic subjects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition 11(3), 501-518. Grosvenor, A., & Lack, L. C. (1984). The effect of sleep before or after learning on memory. Sleep, 7(2), 155-167. Haider, M., Spong, P., & Lindsley, D. B. (1964). Attention, vigilance, and cortical evoked-potentials in humans. Science, 145, 180-182. Hebb, D. O. (1949). The orgnizaion of behavior. New York: John Wiley and Sons.
Hill, H., Ott, F., & Weisbrod, M. (2005). SOA-dependent N400 and P300 semantic priming effects using pseudoword primes and a delayed lexical decision. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 56(3), 209-221. Hill, H., Strube, M., Roesch-Ely, D., & Weisbrod, M. (2002). Automatic vs. controlled processes in semantic priming--differentiation by event-related potentials. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 44(3), 197-218. Hobson, J. A., McCarley, R. W., & Wyzinski, P. W. (1975). Sleep cycle oscillation: reciprocal discharge by two brainstem neuronal groups. Science, 189(4196), 55-58. Holcomb, P. J. (1988). Automatic and attentional processing: an event-related brain potential analysis of semantic priming. Brain and Language, 35(1), 66-85. Holcomb, P. J., & Neville, H. J. (1990). Auditory and visual semantic priming in lexical decision: A comparison using event-related brain potentials. Language and Cognitive Processes, 5(4), 281-312. Jenkins, J. G., & Dallenbach, K. M. (1924). Obliviscence during sleep and waking The American Journal of Psychology, 35(4), 605-612. Kametani, H., & Kawamura, H. (1990). Alterations in acetylcholine release in the rat hippocampus during sleep-wakefulness detected by intracerebral dialysis. Life Sciences, 47(5), 421-426. Karni, A., Tanne, D., Rubenstein, B. S., Askenasy, J. J., & Sagi, D. (1994). Dependence on REM sleep of overnight improvement of a perceptual skill. Science, 265(5172), 679-682. Kounios, J., & Holcomb, P. J. (1992). Structure and process in semantic memory: evidence from event-related brain potentials and reaction times. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 121(4), 459-479. Kuperberg, G. R., Lakshmanan, B. M., Greve, D. N., & West, W. C. (2008). Task and semantic relationship influence both the polarity and localization of hemodynamic modulation during lexico-semantic processing. Human Brain Mapping, 29(5), 544-561. Kuriyama, K., Stickgold, R., & Walker, M. P. (2004). Sleep-dependent learning and motor-skill complexity. Learning & Memory, 11(6), 705-713. Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K. D. (2000). Electrophysiology reveals semantic memory use in language comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(12), 463-470. Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K. D. (2011). Thirty years and counting: finding meaning in the N400 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP). Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 621-647. Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1980). Reading senseless sentences: brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science, 207(4427), 203-205. Kutas, M., & Iragui, V. (1998). The N400 in a semantic categorization task across 6 decades. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 108(5), 456-471. Lau, E. F., Phillips, C., & Poeppel, D. (2008). A cortical network for semantics: (de)constructing the N400. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 9(12), 920-933. Lewin, I., & Glaubman, H. (1975). The effect of REM deprivation: is it detrimental, beneficial, or neutral? Psychophysiology, 12(3), 349-353. Lin, C. C., & Yang, C. M. (2010). Event-related potential indices of new word association following intensive learning of unrelated word-pairs. Poster presented at the 49th Annual Meeting of the Taiwanese Psychological Association. Lund, H. G., Reider, B. D., Whiting, A. B., & Prichard, J. R. (2010). Sleep patterns and predictors of disturbed sleep in a large population of college students. Journal of Adolescent Health, 46(2), 124-132. Lydic, R., & Baghdoyan, H. A. (1998). Handbook of Behavioral State Control: Cellular and Molecular Mechanisms: Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press. Maquet, P. (2001). The role of sleep in learning and memory. Science, 294(5544), 1048-1052. Maquet, P., Laureys, S., Peigneux, P., Fuchs, S., Petiau, C., Phillips, C., et al. (2000). Experience-dependent changes in cerebral activation during human REM sleep. Nature Neuroscience, 3(8), 831-836. Maquet, P., Schwartz, S., Passingham, R., & Frith, C. (2003). Sleep-related consolidation of a visuomotor skill: brain mechanisms as assessed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Journal of Neuroscience, 23(4), 1432-1440. Marrosu, F., Portas, C., Mascia, M. S., Casu, M. A., Fa, M., Giagheddu, M., et al. (1995). Microdialysis measurement of cortical and hippocampal acetylcholine release during sleep-wake cycle in freely moving cats. Brain Research, 671(2), 329-332. Marshall, L., & Born, J. (2007). The contribution of sleep to hippocampus-dependent memory consolidation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(10), 442-450. Mazzoni, G., Gori, S., Formicola, G., Gneri, C., Massetani, R., Murri, L., et al. (1999). Word recall correlates with sleep cycles in elderly subjects. Journal of Sleep Research, 8(3), 185-188. McKone, E., & Slee, J. A. (1997). Explicit contamination in "implicit" memory for new associations. Memory & cognition, 25(3), 352-366. Meienberg, P. (1977). The tonic aspects of human REM sleep during long-term intensive verbal learning. Physiological Psychology, 5, 250-256. Mograss, M., Godbout, R., & Guillem, F. (2006). The ERP old-new effect: A useful indicator in studying the effects of sleep on memory retrieval processes. Sleep, 29(11), 1491-1500. Mograss, M., Guillem, F., Brazzini-Poisson, V., & Godbout, R. (2009). The effects of total sleep deprivation on recognition memory processes: a study of event-related potential. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 91(4), 343-352. Mograss, M., Guillem, F., & Godbout, R. (2008). Event-related potentials differentiates the processes involved in the effects of sleep on recognition memory. Psychophysiology. Näätänen, R., Gaillard, A., & Mäntysalo, S. (1978). The N1 effect of selective attention reinterpreted. Acta Psychologica, 42, 312-329. Neely, J. H. (1991). Semantic priming effects in visual word recognition: a selective review of current findings and theories. In D. Besner, Humphreys, G.W. (Ed.), Basic progresses in reading—visual word recognition (pp. 264-333). NJ: Erlbaum, Hillsdale. Nunez-Pena, M. I., & Honrubia-Serrano, M. L. (2005). N400 and category exemplar associative strength. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 56(1), 45-54. Pace-Schott, E. F., & Hobson, J. A. (2002). The neurobiology of sleep: genetics, cellular physiology and subcortical networks. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 3(8), 591-605. Peigneux, P., Laureys, S., Delbeuck, X., & Maquet, P. (2001). Sleeping brain, learning brain. The role of sleep for memory systems. Neuroreport, 12(18), A111-124. Peigneux, P., Laureys, S., Fuchs, S., Collette, F., Perrin, F., Reggers, J., et al. (2004). Are spatial memories strengthened in the human hippocampus during slow wave sleep? Neuron, 44(3), 535-545. Peters, K. R., Smith, V., & Smith, C. T. (2007). Changes in sleep architecture following motor learning depend on initial skill level. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(5), 817-829. Plihal, W., & Born, J. (1997). Effects of early and late nocturnal sleep on declarative and procedural memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 534-547. Plihal, W., & Born, J. (1999). Effects of early and late nocturnal sleep on priming and spatial memory. Psychophysiology, 36(5), 571-582. Rasch, B., & Born, J. (2007). Maintaining memories by reactivation. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 17(6), 698-703. Rasch, B., Buchel, C., Gais, S., & Born, J. (2007). Odor cues during slow-wave sleep prompt declarative memory consolidation. Science, 315(5817), 1426-1429. Rauchs, G., Desgranges, B., Foret, J., & Eustache, F. (2005). The relationships between memory systems and sleep stages. Journal of Sleep Research, 14(2), 123-140. Rechtschaffen, A., & Kales, A. (1968). A manual of standardized terminology, techniques and scoring systems for sleep stages of human subjects: Los Angeles: Brain Information Service/Brain Research Institute. Rhodes, S. M., & Donaldson, D. I. (2008). Association and not semantic relationships elicit the N400 effect: electrophysiological evidence from an explicit language comprehension task. Psychophysiology, 45(1), 50-59. Roehm, D., Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, I., Rosler, F., & Schlesewsky, M. (2007). To predict or not to predict: influences of task and strategy on the processing of semantic relations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(8), 1259-1274. Rossell, S. L., Price, C. J., & Nobre, A. C. (2003). The anatomy and time course of semantic priming investigated by fMRI and ERPs. Neuropsychologia, 41(5), 550-564. Rudoy, J. D., Voss, J. L., Westerberg, C. E., & Paller, K. A. (2009). Strengthening individual memories by reactivating them during sleep. Science, 326(5956), 1079. Rugg, M. D. (1985). The effects of semantic priming and work repetition on event-related potentials. Psychophysiology, 22(6), 642-647. Rugg, M. D. (1990). Event-related brain potentials dissociate repetition effects of high- and low-frequency words. Memory & Cognition, 18(4), 367-379. Rugg, M. D., & Doyle, M. C. (1994). Event-related potentials and stimulus repetition in indirect and direct. In H. Heinze, T. Munte & G. R. Mangun (Eds.), Cognitive Electrophysiology (pp. 124-148). Boston: Birkhauser. Schacter, D. L., & Buckner, R. L. (1998). Priming and the brain. Neuron, 20(2), 185-195. Schmidt, C., Peigneux, P., Muto, V., Schenkel, M., Knoblauch, V., Munch, M., et al. (2006). Encoding difficulty promotes postlearning changes in sleep spindle activity during napping. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(35), 8976-8982. Sejnowski, T. J., & Destexhe, A. (2000). Why do we sleep? Brain Research, 886(1-2), 208-223. Shima, K., Nakahama, H., & Yamamoto, M. (1986). Firing properties of two types of nucleus raphe dorsalis neurons during the sleep-waking cycle and their responses to sensory stimuli. Brain Research, 399(2), 317-326. Smith, C. (1995). Sleep states and memory processes. Behavioural Brain Research, 69(1-2), 137-145. Smith, C. (2001). Sleep states and memory processes in humans: procedural versus declarative memory systems. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 5(6), 491-506. Smith, C., & Lapp, L. (1991). Increases in number of REMS and REM density in humans following an intensive learning period. Sleep, 14(4), 325-330. Smith, C., & MacNeill, C. (1994). Impaired motor memory for a pursuit rotor task following Stage 2 sleep loss in college students. Journal of Sleep Research, 3(4), 206-213. Smith, C., & Weeden, K. (1990). Post training REMs coincident auditory stimulation enhances memory in humans. Psychiatric Journal of the University of Ottawa, 15(2), 85-90. Smith, C. T., Nixon, M. R., & Nader, R. S. (2004). Posttraining increases in REM sleep intensity implicate REM sleep in memory processing and provide a biological marker of learning potential. Learning & Memory, 11(6), 714-719. Squire, L. R. (1986). Mechanisms of memory. Science, 232(4758), 1612-1619. Squire, L. R., & Zola, S. M. (1996). Structure and function of declarative and nondeclarative memory systems. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 93(24), 13515-13522. Stickgold, R. (1998). Sleep: off-line memory reprocessing. Trends in Cognitive Science, 2(12), 484-492. Stickgold, R. (2004). Dissecting sleep-dependent learning and memory consolidation. Comment on Schabus M et al. Sleep spindles and their significance for declarative memory consolidation. Sleep 2004;27(8):1479-85. Sleep, 27(8), 1443-1445. Stickgold, R. (2005). Sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Nature, 437(7063), 1272-1278. Stickgold, R., James, L., & Hobson, J. A. (2000). Visual discrimination learning requires sleep after training. Nature Neuroscience, 3(12), 1237-1238. Stickgold, R., Scott, L., Rittenhouse, C., & Hobson, J. A. (1999). Sleep-induced changes in associative memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11(2), 182-193. Stickgold, R., Whidbee, D., Schirmer, B., Patel, V., & Hobson, J. A. (2000). Visual discrimination task improvement: A multi-step process occurring during sleep. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(2), 246-254. Su, J.Y. (2010). Study of Promotion of Procedural Memory Consolidation by Auditory Stimulus during Sleep (Unpublished Master`s Thesis). National Cheng Kung University, Tainan,Taiwan. Talamini, L. M., Nieuwenhuis, I. L., Takashima, A., & Jensen, O. (2008). Sleep directly following learning benefits consolidation of spatial associative memory. Learning & Memory, 15(4), 233-237. Tamminen, J., Payne, J. D., Stickgold, R., Wamsley, E. J., & Gaskell, M. G. (2010). Sleep spindle activity is associated with the integration of new memories and existing knowledge. The Journal of Neuroscience, 30(43), 14356-14360. Tilley, A. J., & Empson, J. A. (1978). REM sleep and memory consolidation. Biological Psychology, 6(4), 293-300. Tucker, M. A., Hirota, Y., Wamsley, E. J., Lau, H., Chaklader, A., & Fishbein, W. (2006). A daytime nap containing solely non-REM sleep enhances declarative but not procedural memory. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 86(2), 241-247. Tulving, E. (1972). Episodic and semantic memory: New York: Academic Press. Tulving, E., & Schacter, D. L. (1990). Priming and human memory systems. Science, 247(4940), 301-306. Van Petten, C., Kutas, M., Kluender, R., Mitchiner, M., & Mclsaac, H. (1991). Fractionating the word repetition effect with event-related potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 3(2), 131-150. Velluti, R. A. (1997). Interactions between sleep and sensory physiology. Journal of Sleep Research, 6(2), 61-77. Vertes, R. P. (2004). Memory consolidation in sleep; dream or reality. Neuron, 44(1), 135-148. Vertes, R. P., & Eastman, K. E. (2000). The case against memory consolidation in REM sleep. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23(6), 867-876; discussion 904-1121. Vertes, R. P., & Siegel, J. M. (2005). Time for the sleep community to take a critical look at the purported role of sleep in memory processing. Sleep, 28(10), 1228-1229; discussion 1230-1223. Wagner, A. D., & Koutstaal, W. (2002). Priming. In V. S. Ramachandran (Ed.), Encyclopedia of the Human Brain, Four-Volume Set (Vol. 4, pp. 27-46). New York: Academic Press. Walker, M. P. (2005). A refined model of sleep and the time course of memory formation. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(1), 51-64; discussion 64-104. Walker, M. P., Brakefield, T., Morgan, A., Hobson, J. A., & Stickgold, R. (2002). Practice with sleep makes perfect: sleep-dependent motor skill learning. Neuron, 35(1), 205-211. Walker, M. P., Liston, C., Hobson, J. A., & Stickgold, R. (2002). Cognitive flexibility across the sleep-wake cycle: REM-sleep enhancement of anagram problem solving. Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research, 14(3), 317-324. Walker, M. P., & Stickgold, R. (2006). Sleep, memory, and plasticity. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 139-166. Wamsley, E. J., Tucker, M., Payne, J. D., Benavides, J. A., & Stickgold, R. (2010). Dreaming of a learning task is associated with enhanced sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Current Biology 20(9), 850-855. Yaroush, R., Sullivan, M. J., & Ekstrand, B. R. (1971). Effect of sleep on memory. II. Differential effect of the first and second half of the night. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 88(3), 361-366. Young, M. P., & Rugg, M. D. (1992). Word frequency and multiple repetition as determinants of the modulation of event-related potentials in a semantic classification task. Psychophysiology, 29(6), 664-676. Zola, S. M. (1998). Memory, amnesia, and the issue of recovered memory: neurobiological aspects. Clinical Psychology Review, 18(8), 915-932. |