Loading...
|
Please use this identifier to cite or link to this item:
https://nccur.lib.nccu.edu.tw/handle/140.119/54642
|
Title: | 以事件相關電位探討中文語音辨識中的字形一致性效應 Event-Related Potentials studies for the Orthographic Consistency effects on Chinese spoken word recognition |
Authors: | 陳薇帆 Chen, Wei Fan |
Contributors: | 李佳穎 Lee, Chia Ying 陳薇帆 Chen, Wei Fan |
Keywords: | 中文口語詞彙辨識 音形對應性 同音字密度 字形一致性 |
Date: | 2011 |
Issue Date: | 2012-10-30 11:27:24 (UTC+8) |
Abstract: | 本研究以事件相關電位方法,探討中文字在進行聽覺詞彙辨識的作業中,分別受字形表徵的同音字密度多寡、以及字音-字形對應一致性的影響。首先 進行中文字的字音-字形一致性語料庫的建立,量化中文字的音形對應一致性程度。透過明確的定義並操弄中文字的音形對應一致性,以及中文字同音 字密度的特性,探討在進行中文字聽覺詞彙辨識歷程中,字形表徵屬性如何對語音辨識作業產生影響的認知歷程。實驗分為三種情境(1)同音密度低 (low HD)、(2)同音密度高/音形對應一致性高(high HD/high P-O)、 (3)同音密度高/音形對應一致性低(high HD/low P-O)。前兩項的比較為高同音字密度下的音形對應一致性效果,而後兩項的比較則為在高音形對應一致性下的同音字密度的效果。實驗一採語意判斷作業,研究結果顯示,在高同音字密度時,高音形對應一致性的字引發較大的 N400;而在高音形對應一致性時,同音字密度效果在 LPC 得到顯著的差異。反應中文字在聽 覺詞彙判斷作業上,字形可自動被激活,進而影響語音的辨識。實驗二採押 韻判斷作業,研究結果同樣發現高音形對應一致字引發較大的 N400,實但同音字密度效果性效果在判斷押韻作業上並未達顯著效果。另外,在高音形對應一致性情形下所得到的押韻效果最大,尤以同音字密度高且音形對應一致性高的情況下,押韻效果出現的時間較早。本研究結果支持雙向交互激發模型(BIAM)的假設:中文口語詞彙辨識的歷程中,也會自動激發字形訊息,而語音、字形、語意之間的對應一致性越好,口語詞彙辨識及其整體處理的效能也越高。 |
Reference: | 教育部(民 87)。87 年教育部口語資料庫 。取自 http://www.edu.tw/files/site_content/M0001/86news/ch5-3.html?open 汪舒韻(民 98)。中低頻漢字在唸名與辨識作業中的順向與逆向一致性效果(碩士論文)。 取自 http://ccur.lib.ccu.edu.tw/handle/309310000Q/9282 連育吟(民 99)。中文同音字密度對口語詞彙辨識歷程的影響(碩士論文)。取自 http://ndltd.ncl.edu.tw/cgi- bin/gs32/gsweb.cgi?o=dnclcdr&s=id=%22098NTPTC790001%22.&searchmode=basic Andrews, S. (1982). Phonological recoding: Is the regularity effect consistent? Memory & cognition, 10(6), 565–575. Barber, H., Vergara, M., & Carreiras, M. (2004). Syllable-frequency effects in visual word recognition: evidence from ERPs. Neuroreport, 15(3), 545-548 Bentin, S., Mouchetant-Rostaing, Y., Giard, M., Echallier, J., & Pernier, J. (1999). ERP manifestations of processing printed words at different psycholinguistic levels: time course and scalp distribution. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11(3), 235–260. MIT Press. Danielle van den Brink, Colin M. Brown, and Peter Hagoort (2001). Electrophysiological evidence for early contextual influences during spoken-word recognition: N200 versus N400 effects. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13(7), 967–985. doi:10.1162/089892901753165872 Chen, Y., Lee, J. R., Kuo, W.-J., Hung, D. L., & Cheng, S.-K. (2010). An ERP study of Chinese speakers` rhyme judgments to Chinese and English words. Neuroreport, 21(9), 636–640. doi:10.1097/WNR.0b013e32833a5d2c Coltheart, M., Besner, D., Jonasson, J. T., & Davelaar, E. (1979). Phonological encoding in the lexical decision task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 31(3), 489–507. 61 Coltheart, M., Curtis, B., & Atkins, P. (1993). Models of reading aloud: Dual-route and parallel- distributed-processing approaches Psychological Review. Coltheart, M. (1994). Serial processing in reading aloud: Evidence for dual-route models of reading Journal of Experimental Psychology: .... Coltheart, V., Patterson, K., & Leahy, J. (1994). When a ROWS is a ROSE: Phonological effects in written word comprehension. The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology A, Human experimental psychology, 47(4), 917–955. Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Ziegler, J. C., Langdon, R. (2001).DRC: A Dual Route Cascaded Model of Visual Word Recognition and ReadingAloud., Psychological Review, Vol. 108, No. I, 204- 256 Friedman, D. D., & Johnson, R. R. (2000). Event-related potential (ERP) studies of memory encoding and retrieval: a selective review. Microscopy Research and Technique, 51(1), 6–28. doi:10.1002/1097-0029(20001001)51:1<6::AID-JEMT2>3.0.CO;2-R Friedrich, M., Friederici, A.D.(2006).Early N400 development and later language acquisition., Psychophysiology, 43. Grainger, Jonathan.(1994) Phonology and orthography in visual word recognition: Effects of masked homophone primes , Journal of Memory and Language, 33:2 p.218 Grainger, J., & Ziegler, J. (2008). Cross-code consistency in a functional architecture for word recognition. Single-word reading: Behavioral and biological perspectives, 129–158. Glushko, R. (1979). The organization and activation of orthographic knowledge in reading aloud, Journal ofExperimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 5, 674-691 Holcomb, P. J., & Neville, H. J. (1990). Auditory and visual semantic priming in lexical decision: A comparison using event-related brain potentials. LANGUAGE AND COGNITIVE PROCESSES, 5(4), 281–312. doi:10.1080/01690969008407065 62 Holcomb, P. J., & Neville, H. J. (1991). Natural speech processing: An analysis using event-related brain potentials. Holcomb, P. J. (1993). Semantic priming and stimulus degradation: implications for the role of the N400 in language processing. Psychophysiology, 30(1), 47-61. Hillyard, S. A., Hink, R. F., Schwent, V. L., & Picton, T. W. (1973). Electrical Signs of Selective Attention in the Human Brain. Science, 182(4108), 177-180. Hue, C.-W., (1992). Recognition processes in character naming. In Language processing in Chinese. Advances in psychology. Vol. 90, H.-C.E. Chen, Tzeng, Ovid J L., eds. Amsterdam, Netherlands: North-Holland. xii, 394, pp. 93-107. Jacquier, C., Rouibah, A., & Hoen, M. (2005). ERP Evidence for an Interaction between Phonological and Semantic Processes in Masked Priming Tasks. Proceedings of the XXVIIth annual meeting of the cognitive science society (CogSci 2005), 1030–1035. Kutas, M. and Hillyard, S. A. (1980). Reading senseless sentences: brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science, 207, 203-208. Kutas, M. and Hillyard, S. A. An electrophysiological probe of incidental semantic association, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 1989, 1(1): 38-49. Lee, C.Y., Tsai, J.L., Kuo, W.J., Yeh, T.C., Wu, Y.T., Ho, L.T., Hung, D.L., Tzeng, O.J.&Hsieh, J.C., (2004). Neuronal correlates of consistency and frequency effects on Chinese character naming: an event-related fMRI study. NeuroImage. 23, 1235-45. Lee, C.Y., Tsai, J.L., Su, E.C.I., Tzeng, O.J.L.&Hung, D.L., (2005). Consistency, Regularity, and Frequency Effects in Naming Chinese Characters. Language and Linguistics. 6, 75-107. 63 Lee, C.Y., Tsai, J.L., Huang, H.W., Hung, D.L.&Tzeng, O.J.L., (2006). The temporal signatures of semantic and phonological activations for Chinese sublexical processing: An event-related potential study. Brain Research. 1121, 150-159. Lee, C., Tsai, J., Chan, W., Hsu, C., & Hung, D. (2007). Temporal dynamics of the consistency effect in reading Chinese: an event-related potentials study. Neuroreport. Lee, C., Huang, H., Kuo, W., & Tsai, J. (2010). Cognitive and neural basis of the consistency and lexicality effects in reading Chinese. Journal of Neurolinguistics. Liu, Y., Perfetti, C. A., & Hart, L. (2003). ERP evidence for the time course of graphic, phonological, and semantic information in Chinese meaning and pronunciation decisions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29(6), 1231-1247. Nagi, K. (2008). The effect of feedback consistency and feedforward consistency (number of homophones and consistency) on reading and writing of Chinese in school-aged children, 84. Paller, K. A., Kutas, M., & McIsaac, H. K. (1995). Monitoring conscious recollection via the electrical activity of the brain. Psychological science : a journal of the American Psychological Society / APS, 6(2), 107. Pattamadilok, C., Perre, L., Dufau, S., Ziegler, J.C. (2009). On-line orthographic influences on spoken language in a semantic task. J Cogn Neurosci. 21, 169-79. Pattamadilok, C., Perre, L., & Ziegler, J. C. (2011). Beyond rhyme or reason: ERPs reveal task-specific activation of orthography on spoken language. BRAIN AND LANGUAGE, 116(3), 116–124. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2010.12.002 Perre, L., & Ziegler, J. C. (2008). On-line activation of orthography in spoken word recognition. Brain Research, 1188, 132–138. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2007.10.084 Perre, L., Pattamadilok, C., Montant, M., Ziegler, J.C., 2009b. Orthographic effects in spoken language: on-line activation or phonological restructuring? Brain Research. 1275, 73-80. 64 Pattamadilok, C., Perre, L., & Ziegler, J. C. (2011). Beyond rhyme or reason: ERPs reveal task-specific activation of orthography on spoken language. BRAIN AND LANGUAGE, 116(3), 116–124. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2010.12.002 Rugg MD. (1984). Event-related potentials in phonological matching tasks. Brain Lang, 23, 225–240. Rugg, M. D., Mark, R. E., Walla, P., Schloerscheidt, A. M., Birch, C. S., & Allan, K. (1998). Dissociation of the neural correlates of implicit and explicit memory. Nature, 392(6676), 595–598. doi:10.1038/33396 Seidenberg, M. S. (1985). The time course of phonological code activation in two writing systems. Cognition, 19(1), 1–30. Seidenberg, M. (1989). A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming Psychological Review, 97. Seidenberg, M. S., & McClelland, J. L. (1989). A Distributed, Developmental Model of Word Recognition and Naming: Defense Technical Information Center. Smith, M. E., & Guster, K. (1993). Decomposition of recognition memory event-related potentials yields target, repetition, and retrieval effects. Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology, 86(5), 335–343. Van Orden, G. C., Johnston, J. C., & Hale, B. L. (1988). Word identification in reading proceeds from spelling to sound to meaning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14(3), 371–386. Van Orden, G. C. (1987). A ROWS is a ROSE: spelling, sound, and reading. Memory & cognition, 15(3), 181–198. Ventura, P., Morais, J., Pattamadilok, C., Kolinsky, R., 2004. The locus of the orthographic consistency effect in auditory word recognition. Lang. Cogn. Processes 19, 57–95. 65 Van Petten, C., Coulson, S., Rubin, S., Plante, E., & Parks, M. (1999). Time course of word identification and semantic integration in spoken language Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25(2), 394–417. Vitevitch, M. S., & Luce, P. A. (1999). Probabilistic Phonotactics and Neighborhood Activation in Spoken Word Recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 40(3), 374–408. doi:10.1006/jmla.1998.2618 Wang, W., Li, X., Ning, N., & Zhang, J. X. (2012). The nature of the homophone density effect: an ERP study with Chinese spoken monosyllable homophones. Neuroscience letters, 516(1), 67–71. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2012.03.059 Yoncheva, Y. N., Zevin, J. D., Maurer, U., & McCandliss, B. D. (2010). Auditory selective attention to speech modulates activity in the visual word form area Cerebral Cortex, 20(3), 622–632. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhp129 Ziegler, J. C, Muneaux, M. & Grainger, J . (2003). Neighborhood effects in auditory word recognition: Phonological competition and orthographic facilitation. Journal of Memory & Language, 48, 779-793. Ziegler, J. C., & Ferrand, L. (1998). Orthography shapes the perception of speech: The consistency effect in auditory recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5, 683–689. Ziegler, J. C., Ferrand, L., & Montant, M. (2004). Visual phonology: The effects of orthographic consistency on different auditory word recognition tasks. Memory & Cognition, 32, 732–741. Ziegler, J. C., & Muneaux, M. (2007). Orthographic facilitation and phonological inhibition in spoken word recognition: A developmental study. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 75–80. Ziegler, J. C., Muneaux, M., & Grainger, J. (2003). Neighborhood effect in auditory word recognition: Phonological competition and orthographic facilitation. Journal of Memory and Language, 48, 779– 793. |
Description: | 碩士 國立政治大學 心理學研究所 98752023 100 |
Source URI: | http://thesis.lib.nccu.edu.tw/record/#G0098752023 |
Data Type: | thesis |
Appears in Collections: | [心理學系] 學位論文
|
Files in This Item:
File |
Size | Format | |
202301.pdf | 3085Kb | Adobe PDF2 | 1812 | View/Open |
|
All items in 政大典藏 are protected by copyright, with all rights reserved.
|