Loading...
|
Please use this identifier to cite or link to this item:
https://nccur.lib.nccu.edu.tw/handle/140.119/31018
|
Title: | 台灣期貨交易所選擇權投資人的交易行為 The Trading Behavior of Options Investors at Taiwan Futures Exchange |
Authors: | 王銘駿 Wang, Ming Chun |
Contributors: | 劉玉珍 Liu,Yu Jane 王銘駿 Wang, Ming Chun |
Keywords: | 行為財務學 框架效應 心理帳戶 Behavioral Finance Narrow Framing Mental Accounting |
Date: | 2006 |
Issue Date: | 2009-09-14 09:04:09 (UTC+8) |
Abstract: | 在第一篇文章當中,我們論證了台灣選擇權市場的投資人交易行為符合Kahneman and Tversky (1981) 所提出來的框架效應. 更進一步, 我們發現投資人的專業程度, 交易複雜性, 與交易經驗能降低框架效應的行為偏誤.
在第二篇文章當中, 我們發現台灣選擇權市場投資人並無符合Thaler (1985) 所提出來的心理帳戶效應來編輯利得與損失. 但是對於混合利得與損失當中, 投資人仍有心理帳戶的現象來處理他們的選擇權部位. In the first essay, we document support for the narrow framing effect proposed by Kahneman and Tversky (1981). Our findings that traders in an options market frame complicated investment decisions into the simpler ones support the narrow framing effect. Traders’ professionalism, sophistication and trading experience are negatively related with the degree of narrow framing, implying that these factors help to reduce investors’ behavioral bias. Our study bridges the gap between the psychological literature and financial literature in terms of the relationship between experience/sophistication and narrow framing. The results of this paper shed light on the decision-making process in an options market.
In the second essay, complementary to Lim (2006)’s findings in regards to stocks market, we also claim that in a much more complex derivatives market, traders tend to frame gains and losses asymmetrically by editing or evaluating their outcomes into different accounts. Nevertheless, different from Thaler’s mental accounting theory (1985), we find investors are more susceptible to segregating losses and integrating gains when they liquidate their positions. Our empirical evidence shows that they also have asymmetry in the propensity to liquidate multiple options. The current study sheds a light on how investors perceive, categorize, evaluate and engage their outcomes in financial activities, in addition, under what circumstances investor integrate or separate their investment profits. The fact that investors’ responses to edit their outcomes vary across countries and securities markets highlights the complexity of human behavior and calls for further studies on a broader range of financial markets. |
Reference: | Bailey, T. C. and A. C. Gatrell, (1995), Interactive Spatial Data Analysis (Longman Scientific & Technical: Essex, England). Barber, B., Lee, Y., Liu Y. J., and Odean, T., (2007) “Just How Much Do Individual Investors Lose by Trading?” Working paper. Barberis, N. and Huang, M., (2001), “Mental Accounting, Loss Aversion and Individual Stock Returns,” Journal of Finance 56, 1247-1292. Barberis, N., Huang, M., and Santos, T., (2001), “Prospect Theory and Asset Prices,” Quarterly Journal of Economics 116, 1-53. Barberis, N. and Huang, M., (2004), “Preferences with Frames: A New Utility Specification that Allows for the Framing of Risks,” Working paper, University of Chicago. Barberis, N., Huang, M., and Thaler, R., (2006) “Individual Preferences, Monetary Gambles, and Stock Market Participation: A Case for Narrow Framing,” American Economic Review 96, 1069-1090. Benartzi, S. and Thaler, R. H., (1995), ‘‘Myopic Loss Aversion and the Equity Premium Puzzle,’’ The Quarterly Journal of Economics 110, 73-92. Bloomfield, R., Libby, R., and Nelson, M., (1998), “Confidence and the Welfare of Less-Informed Investors,” Working Paper, Cornell University. Brennan, M. and Solanki, R., (1981), “Optimal Portfolio Insurance,” Journal of Financial and Quantitative Analysis 16, 279-300. Brown, P., Chappel, N., da Silva Rosa, R., Walter, T., (2003), “The Reach of the Disposition Effect: Large Sample Evidence across Investor Classes,” Working paper, University of Western Australia. Chaput, J. S. and Ederington, L., (2003), “Option Spread and Combination Trading,” Journal of Derivatives 10, 70-88. Chaput, J. S. and Ederington, L., (2005a), “Volatility Spread Design,” Journal of Futures Markets, vol 25, #3 (March), 243-279. Chaput, J. S. and Ederington, L., (2005b), “Vertical Spread Design,” Journal of Derivatives, vol 12, #3 (Spring), 28-46. Cho, J., and Jo, K., (2006), “Overconfidence and Information: The Differences between Individuals and Institutions,” Working paper, Seoul National University. Chung, C. F., and Yeh, C. Y., (2005), “Strategic Asset Allocation under Narrow Framing / Loss Aversion and Volatility Feedback,” Working paper, Taiwan University. Cox, John C., and Mark Rubinstein (1985), Options Markets, Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ. Dhar, Ravi, and Zhu, N., (2003), “Up, close and personal: An individual level analysis of the disposition effect,” Working Paper, International Center for Finance, Yale University. Doran, J. and Hamernick, R., (2006), “Is There Money to Be Made Investing in Options? A Historical Perspective,” Working Paper, Florida University. Driessen, J. and Maenhout, P., (2004), “A Portfolio Perspective on Option Pricing Anomalies,” Working Paper, Amsterdam University. Fagley, N. S., and Kruger, L., (1986), “The Effect of Problem Framing on the Program Choice of School Psychologists,” Paper presented at the 94th Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association, August, Washington, DC. Feng, Lei, and Seasholes, Mark S., (2005), “Do Investor Sophistication and Trading Experience Eliminate Behavioral Biases in Financial Markets?,” Review of Finance 9, 305–351. Ferris, S. P., R. A. Haugen, and A. K. Makhija, (1988), “Predicting Contemporary Volume with Historic Volume at Differential Price Levels: Evidence Supporting the Disposition Effect,” Journal of Finance 43, 677-697. Frazzini, A., (2003), “Narrow Framing and Limit to Arbitrage,” Working Paper, International Center for Finance, Yale University. Grinblatt, M., and Han, B., (2005), “Prospect theory, mental accounting, and momentum,” Journal of Financial Economics 78, 311-339. Grinblatt, M., and Keloharju M., (2000), “The investment behavior and performance of various investor types: a study of Finland`s unique data set,” Journal of Financial Economics 55, 43-67. Grinblatt, M., and Keloharju M., (2001a), “How Distance, Language and Culture Influence Stockholdings and Trades,” Journal of Finance 56, 1053-1073. Grinblatt, M., and Keloharju, M., (2001b), “What Makes Investor Trade?” Journal of Finance 56, 2, 589-616. Haliassos, M. and Hassapis, C., (2001), “Non-expected Utility, Saving, and Portfolios,” Economic Journal 111, 69-102. Haigh, Michael and List, John A, (2005), “Do Professional Traders Exhibit Myopic Loss Aversion? An Experimental Analysis,” Journal of Finance, 60 (1): 523-534. He, P. and Hu, X., (2007), “Household Investment—the Horizon Effect,” Working Paper, University of Illinois at Chicago. Heath, C., S. Huddart and M. Lang, (1999), “Psychological Factors and Stock Option Exercise,” The Quarterly Journal of Economics, Vol. 114, No. 2., 601-627. Hull, John C., (2003), Options, Futures, and Other Derivatives (5th ed.), Prentice Hall, Upper Saddle River, NJ. Kahneman, D. and Tversky, A., (1984), “Choices, Values, and Frames,” American Psychologist 39, 341–350. Kahneman, D., Tversky, A., (1979), “Prospect Theory: An Analysis of Decision under Risk,” Econometrica 47, 263–291. Kahneman, D. and Lovallo, D., (1993), ‘‘Timid Choices and Bold Forecasts: A Cognitive Perspective on Risk Taking,’’ Management Science 39, 17-31. Kaustia, M., (2005), “Mental accounting of stock market profits: the ‘buy low sell high’ heuristic,” Unpublished working paper, Helsinki School of Economics. Koch, A. K., and Lazarov, Z., (2001), “Clustering of Trading Activity in the DAX Index Options Market,” Working paper, Bonn University. Knez, Peter, Vernon L. Smith and Arlington W. Williams, (1985), “Individual Rationality, Market Rationality, and Value Estimation,” American Economics Review 75, 397-402. Kühberger, A., (1998), “The Influence of Framing on Risky Decisions: A Meta-analysis,” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 75, 23–55. Kühberger, A., (1995), “The framing of decisions: A New Look at Old Problems,” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 62, 230–240. Kumar, A. and Lim, S., (2006), “How do Decision Frames Influence the Stock Investment Choices of Individual Investors?” Working Paper, Notre Dame University Lakonishok, J., Lee, I., Pearson, N., and Poteshman, A., (2006), “Option market Activity,” Working Paper, Illinois University at Urbana-Champaign. Leland, H., (1980), “Who Should Buy Portfolio Insurance?,” Journal of Finance 35, 581-596. Levin, I. P., Schneider, S., and Gaeth, G. J., (1998), “All Frames Are Not Created Equal: A Typology and Critical Analysis of Framing Effects,” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 76, 149–188. Lim, Sonya Seongyeon, (2006), “Do investors integrate losses and segregate gains? Mental accounting and investor trading decisions,” Journal of Business 79(5), 2539-2573. List, John A., (2003), “Does Market Experience Eliminate Market Anomalies?,” Quarterly Journal of Economics, 118(1), 41-71. Liu, J. and Pan, J., (2003), “Dynamic Derivative Strategies,” Journal of Financial Economics 69, 401-430. Liu, Y. J., Tsai, C. L., Wang, M. C. and Zhu, N., (2006), “House Money Effect: Evidence from Market Makers at Taiwan Futures Exchange,” Working Paper, Chengchi University. Liu, Y. J., and Wang, M. C., (2007), “Narrow Framing: Professions, Sophistication and Experience,” Working Paper, Chengchi University. Ljungqvist, A. and Wilhelm, W., (2005), “Does prospect theory explain IPO market behavior?,” Journal of Finance 60, 1759-1790. Loke, W. H., and Lau, S. L. L., (1992), “Effects of Framing and Mathematical Experience on Judgments,” Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 30, 393-395. Loke, W. H., and Tan, K. F., (1992), “Effects of Framing and Missing Information in Expert and Novice Judgment,” Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 30, 187-190. Loughran,T., and Ritter, J., (2002), “Why don’t issuers get upset about leaving money on the table in IPOs?,” Review of Financial Studies 15,413–443. Loughran, T., and Ritter, J., (2004), “Why has IPO underpricing changed over time?, “ Financial Management 33, 5–37. MacDonald, Robert L., (2003), Derivatives Markets, Addison Wesley, New York, NY. Magi Alessandro (2005), “Financial Decision-making and Portfolio Choice under Behavioral Preferences: Implications for the Equity Home Bias Puzzle,” Working paper, University of Bologna. Mankiw, N.G., and Zeldes, S., (1991), “The Consumption of Stockholders and Non-stockholders,” Journal of Financial Economics 29, 97-112. Moon, P., Keasey, K., and Duxbury, D. (1999), “Mental accounting and decision making: The relationship between relative and absolute savings,” Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 38, 145–153. Mowen, M. M., & Mowen, J. C. (1986), “An empirical examination of the biasing effects of framing on business decisions,” Decision Sciences, 17, 596–602. Nicolosi, Peng, G., L., and Zhu, N., (2004), “Do Individual Investors Learn from Their Trading Experience?,” Yale ICF Working Paper No. 03-32. Odean, T., (1998), “Are investors reluctant to realize their losses?” Journal of Finance 53,1775–1798. O`Curry, S. 1999. Consumer budgeting and mental accounting. In The Elgar Companion to Con-sumer Research and Economic Psychology, edited by P. E. Earl and S. Kemp, XX-XX. Northhampton, MA: Cheltenham. Poteshman, A., M., (2001), “Underreaction, Overreaction, and Increasing Misreaction to Information in the Options Market,” Journal of Finance, Vol. 56, No. 3, June 2001, 851-876. Rantapuska, E., (2005), “Do investors reinvest dividends and tender offer proceeds? “ Unpublished working paper, Helsinki School of Economics. Ranyard, R., & Abdel-Nabi, D. (1993), “Mental accounting and the process of multiattribute choice,” Acta Psychologica, 84, 161–177. Read, D., and Loewenstein, G., (1995), ‘‘Diversification Bias: Explaining the Discrepancy in Variety Seeking between Combined and Separated Choices,’’ Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied 1, 1, 34-49. Read, D., Loewenstein, G. and Kalyanaraman, S., (1999), ‘‘Mixing Virtue and Vice: The Combined Effects of Hyperbolic Discounting and Diversification,’’ Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 12, 257-273. Redelmeier, D. A., and Tversky, A., (1992), “On the Framing of Multiple Prospects,” Psychological Science, 3, 191-193. Ritchken, Peter, (1996), Derivatives Markets: Theory, Strategy, and Applications, Harper Collins Publishers, NY. Rockenbach, B., (2004), “The behavioral relevance of mental accounting for the pricing of financial options,” Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization 53, 513-527. Roszkowski, M. J., and Snelbecker, G. E., (1990), “Effects of “Framing” on Measure of Risk: Financial Planners Are not Immune,” The Journal of Behavioral Economics 19, 237-246. Sautner, Z., and Weber, M., (2006), “How Do Managers Behave in Stock Option Plans? Evidence from Exercise and Survey Data,” Working Paper, the Saïd Business School, University of Oxford. Seru, A., Shumway, T., and Stoffman, N., (2006), “Learning by Trading,” Working Paper, University of Michigan. Shapira, Z., and Venezia, I., (2000), “Patterns of Behavior of Professionally Managed and Independent Investors,” Journal of Banking and Finance, July, 1-15. Shefrin, H. and Statman, M., (1984), “Explaining investor preference for cash dividends,” Journal of Financial Economics 13, 253–282. Shefrin, H., Statman, M, (1985), “The disposition effect to sell winners too early and ride losers too long,” Journal of Finance 40, 777–790. Shefrin, H. and Thaler, R., (1988), “The behavioral life-cycle hypothesis,” Economic Inquiry 26, 609–643. Simonson, I. (1990), ‘‘The Effect of Purchase Quantity and Timing on Variety Seeking Behavior,’’ Journal of Marketing Research 32, 150-162. Stoll, H. and Whaley, R., (1993), Futures and Options: Theory and Applications, Southwestern Publishing, Cincinnati, OH. Thaler, R., (1980), “Toward a positive theory of consumer choice,” Journal of Economic Behaviorand Organization 1, 39–60. Thaler, R., Shefrin, H., (1981), “An economic theory of self-control,” Journal of Political Economy 89, 392–406. Thaler, R., (1985), “Mental Accounting and Consumer Choice,” Marketing Science 4, 199–214. Thaler, R., and Johnson, E., (1990), “Gambling with the house money and trying to break even: The effects of prior outcomes on risky choice,” Management Science 36, 643–660. Thaler, R., (1994), “Psychology and savings policies,” American Economic Review Papers and Proceedings 84, 186–192. Thaler, R., (1999), “Mental accounting matters,” Journal of Behavior Decision Making 12: 183-206. Thaler, R. (2003), Mental accounting matters. In Advances in Behavioral Economics, edited by C. F. Camerer, G. Loewenstein, and M. Rabin, 75-103. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Tversky, A. and Kahneman, D., (1981), “The Framing Effect of Decisions and the Psychology of Choice,” Science 211, 453–458. Tversky, A. and Kahneman, D., (1986), “Rational Choice and the Framing of Decisions,” Journal of Business 59, S252–S278. Veld, C. and Horst, J. T. (2003), “Behavioral Preferences for Individual Securities: The Case for Call Warrants and Call Options,” Working Paper, University of Sterling. |
Description: | 博士 國立政治大學 財務管理研究所 90357501 95 |
Source URI: | http://thesis.lib.nccu.edu.tw/record/#G0903575011 |
Data Type: | thesis |
Appears in Collections: | [財務管理學系] 學位論文
|
Files in This Item:
File |
Size | Format | |
index.html | 0Kb | HTML2 | 298 | View/Open |
|
All items in 政大典藏 are protected by copyright, with all rights reserved.
|