English  |  正體中文  |  简体中文  |  Post-Print筆數 : 27 |  Items with full text/Total items : 113148/144119 (79%)
Visitors : 50707680      Online Users : 302
RC Version 6.0 © Powered By DSPACE, MIT. Enhanced by NTU Library IR team.
Scope Tips:
  • please add "double quotation mark" for query phrases to get precise results
  • please goto advance search for comprehansive author search
  • Adv. Search
    HomeLoginUploadHelpAboutAdminister Goto mobile version
    政大機構典藏 > 商學院 > 資訊管理學系 > 學位論文 >  Item 140.119/112160
    Please use this identifier to cite or link to this item: https://nccur.lib.nccu.edu.tw/handle/140.119/112160


    Title: 以推敲可能性理論探討軟體專案承諾升級
    Escalation of Commitment in Software Projects: An Elaboration Likelihood Model Perspective
    Authors: 張菀庭
    Chang, Wan-Ting
    Contributors: 梁定澎
    周彥君

    Liang, Ting-Peng
    Chou, Yen-Chun

    張菀庭
    Chang, Wan-Ting
    Keywords: 軟體專案管理
    承諾升級
    推敲可能性理論
    訊息訴求
    訊息框架
    Software project management
    Escalation of commitment
    Elaboration likelihood model
    Message appealing
    Framing effect
    Date: 2017
    Issue Date: 2017-08-28 11:27:55 (UTC+8)
    Abstract: 軟體專案承諾升級現象發生時,可能會造成更多資源成本的投入,若投入更多的資源,而專案依然失敗,則會造成更多的浪費,是企業界最不希望看到的情形。本研究利用推敲可能性理論探討此現象,了解不同的說服方式如何影響受測者的決策過程。根據遇到專案的狀況,依照訊息訴求分成理性訴求與感性訴求的敘述方式,加入框架效應 (正向/負向) 以及訊息強度 (低/中/高) 兩變數,以自我責任作為調節變項進行探討,研究在不同情況敘述下,決策制定者接收專案訊息描述後其決策過程中的推敲可能性,以及推敲可能性與決策之間的關係。
    研究發現如下:
    1. 單一效果影響下,訊息強度中比起其他兩個強度較容易引起訊息接收者運用中央路徑思考。
    2. 訊息訴求、訊息框架以及訊息強度會交互影響受測者推敲可能性,在正向框架下,訊息訴求與訊息強度對訊息接收者運用中央路徑做決策有顯著的交互作用。
    3. 在訊息強度中與強的情況下訊息訴求與訊息框架對訊息接收者運用中央路徑做決策有顯著的交互作用。
    4. 在感性訴求、正向框架與訊息強度強的訊息描述下,訊息接收者運用中央路徑思考時,較不容易做出承諾升級的決定。
    5. 在自我責任調節下,則是感性訴求、負向框架與訊息強度弱的訊息描述,會引起訊息接收者運用中央路徑思考時,較不容易做出專案繼續的決策。
    Escalation of commitment is common in software project development. There are a few theories that have been used to explain this behavior, including the framing effect and self-responsibility. This study investigates the issue from the dual-path elaboration likelihood model (ELM) to examine how different persuasion routes may play roles in the decision process. An experiment was designed to study the effect of different descriptions of project status that may lead to different decision routes (central versus peripheral routes). The experiment design includes message appealing (rational vs emotional appealing), message strength (strong, medium and weak), and framing (positive vs. negative) as main variables and the responsibility as a moderator. The
    subject was asked to decide whether s/he would continue the project under a given scenario. Our results includes the following:
    1. Message appealing, message framing, and message strength have significant interaction effect on the subject’s decision routes;
    2. In positive framing, message appealing and strength has significant interaction effect on the use of the central route;
    3. When message strength is medium or strong, message appealing and framing has significant interaction effect on the use of the central route;
    4. Regarding to decision escalation, the likelihood of escalation is lower when the decision route is central (thinking) under the emotional appealing, positive framing, and strong message;
    5. The likelihood of escalation is lower when the decision route is central under the emotional appealing, negative framing, and weak message description.
    Reference: 一、中文部分
    王凱、王存國與范錚強 (民 95)。線上環境中廣告情境呈現與執行手法對廣告效果的影響:廣告變化,訊息訴求與導引效果。資訊管理學報,13 卷,3 期,1-28 頁。
    余曉玟 (民 105)。軟體專案承諾升級─沉沒成本與趨避理論的觀點 (未出版之碩士論文)。國立政治大學,臺北市。
    祝鳳岡 (民 84)。廣告感性訴求策略之策略分析。廣告學研究,5,85-112 頁。
    祝鳳岡 (民 85)。廣告理性訴求策略之策略分析。廣告學研究,8,1-26 頁。
    梁定澎 (民 101)。資訊管理理論。新北市:前程文化。
    彭仁柏 (民 105)。軟體專案承諾升級的研究─自我辯護理論與框架效應 (未出版之碩士論文)。國立政治大學,臺北市。
    二、英文部分
    Arkes, H., Blumer, C. (1985). The Psychology of Sunk Cost. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 35(1), 124-140.
    Bargh, J. A., Chaiken, S., Govender, R., & Pratto, F. (1992). The generality of the automatic attitude activation effect. Journal of personality and social psychology, 62(6), 893-912.
    Bhattacherjee, A., & Sanford, C. (2006). Influence processes for information technology acceptance: An elaboration likelihood model. MIS quarterly, 30(4), 805-825.
    Brockner, J. (1992). The escalation of commitment to a failing course of action: Toward theoretical progress. Academy of management Review, 17(1), 39-61.
    Brockner, J., & Rubin, J. Z. (2012). Entrapment in escalating conflicts: A social psychological analysis. Springer Science & Business Media.
    Burgoon, M. (1989). Messages and persuasive effects. In J. J. Bradac (Ed.), Message effects in communication science. (pp. 129-164). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
    Cacioppo, J. T., & Petty, R. E. (1989). Effects of message repetition on argument processing, recall, and persuasion. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 10(1), 3-12.
    Camerer, C. F. (2003). Strategizing in the brain. Science, 300(5626), 1673-1675.
    Conner, M., Sparks, P., Povey, R., James, R., & Shepherd, R. (1996). AttitudeIntention-Behavior links: Moderating role of attitudinal ambivalence. In 11th General Meeting of the European Association of Experimental Social Psychology,Gmunden, Austria.
    Coulter, K. S., & Punj, G. N. (2007). Understanding the role of idiosyncratic thinking in brand attitude formation: the dual interference model. Journal of Advertising, 36(1), 7-20.
    Desai, M. S., & Chulkov, D.V. (2009). Escalation of Commitment in MIS Projects: A Meta-Analysis, International Journal of Management & Information Systems, 13(2), 29-38.
    Dijkstra, J. J. (1999). User agreement with incorrect expert system advice. Behaviour & Information Technology, 18(6), 399-411.
    Evans, J. S. B. (2008). Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition. Annu. Rev. Psychol., 59, 255-278.
    Garland, H., Sandefur, C. A., & Rogers, A. C. (1990). De-escalation of commitment in oil exploration: When sunk costs and negative feedback coincide. Journal of Applied Psychology, 75(6), 721-727.
    Groups, S. (2014). Chaos report 2004.
    Hackman, J. R., & Oldham, G. R. (1980). Work redesign.
    Herr, P. M., Kardes, F. R., & Kim, J. (1991). Effects of word-of-mouth and productattribute information on persuasion: An accessibility-diagnosticity perspective. Journal of consumer research, 17(4), 454-462.
    Keil, M., & Flatto, J. (1999). Information systems project escalation: a reinterpretation based on options theory. Accounting, Management and Information Technologies, 9(2), 115-139.
    Keil, M., & Robey, D. (1999). Turning around troubled software projects: An exploratory study of the deescalation of commitment to failing courses of action.
    Journal of Management Information Systems, 15(4), 63-87.
    Kempf, D. S., & Palan, K. M. (2006). The effects of gender and argument strength on the processing of word-of-mouth communication. Academy of Marketing Studies Journal, 10(1), 1-18.
    Kotler, P., Keller, K. L., Manceau, D., & Hémonnet-Goujot, A. (2015). Marketing management (Vol. 14). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
    Lee, J., Park, D. H., & Han, I. (2008). The effect of negative online consumer reviews on product attitude: An information processing view. Electronic commerce research and applications, 7(3), 341-352.
    Levin, I. P., Schneider, S. L., & Gaeth, G. J. (1998). All frames are not created equal: A typology and critical analysis of framing effects. Organizational behavior and human decision processes, 76(2), 149-188.
    Maheswaran, D., & Chaiken, S. (1991). Promoting systematic processing in lowmotivation settings: effect of incongruent information on processing and judgment. Journal of personality and social psychology, 61(1), 13-25.
    Meyerowitz, B. E., & Chaiken, S. (1987). The effect of message framing on breast selfexamination attitudes, intentions, and behavior. Journal of personality and social psychology, 52(3), 500-510.
    Montealegre, R., & Keil, M. (2000). De-escalating information technology projects: Lessons from the Denver International Airport. MIS Quarterly, 24(3), 417-447.
    Norman, R. (1975). Affective-cognitive consistency, attitudes, conformity, and behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32(1), 83-91.
    Nylen, D. W. (1986). Advertising: planning, implementation, and control, SouthWestern Publishing Company, 427-428.
    Petty, R. E. & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986b). Communication and persuasion: Central and peripheral route to attitude change. New York: Springer-Verlag.
    Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986a). The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion.
    In Communication and persuasion, 1-24. Springer New York.
    Petty, R. E., & Wegener, D. T. (1998). Matching versus mismatching attitude functions: Implications for scrutiny of persuasive messages. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24(3), 227-240.
    Ray, M. L. (1982). Advertising and communication management, Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 247-272.
    Rosenberg, M. J. (1968). Hedonism, inauthenticity, and other goads toward expansion of a consistency theory. Theories of cognitive consistency: A sourcebook, 73-111.
    Sengupta, J., & Johar, G. V. (2002). Effects of inconsistent attribute information on the predictive value of product attitudes: Toward a resolution of opposing perspectives. Journal of Consumer research, 29(1), 39-56.
    Shimp, T. A. (1990). Promotion management and marketing communications, Chicago: Dryden Press, 343-353.
    Singh, S.N. and Dalal, N.P. (1999). Web Home Pages as Advertisements, Communications of the ACM, 42(8), 91-98.
    Srull, T. K., & Wyer, R. S. (1989). Person memory and judgment. Psychological review, 96(1), 58-63.
    Staw, B. M., & Ross, J. (1980). Commitment in an experimenting society: A study of the attribution of leadership from administrative scenarios. Journal of Applied Psychology, 65(3), 249-260.
    Staw, B. M., (1976) Knee-Deep in the Big Muddy: A Study of Escalating Commitment to a Chosen Course of Action. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 16, 27-44.
    Sussman, S. W., & Siegal, W. S. (2003). Informational influence in organizations: An integrated approach to knowledge adoption. Information systems research, 14(1), 47-65.
    Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science, 453-458.
    Warden, C. A., Wu, W. Y., & Tsai, D. (2006). Online shopping interface components: Relative importance as peripheral and central cues. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 9(3), 285-296.
    Whyte, G. (1986). Escalation Commitment to a Course of Action: A Reinterpretation. Academy of Management Review, 11(2), 311-321.
    Description: 碩士
    國立政治大學
    資訊管理學系
    104356030
    Source URI: http://thesis.lib.nccu.edu.tw/record/#G0104356030
    Data Type: thesis
    Appears in Collections:[資訊管理學系] 學位論文

    Files in This Item:

    File SizeFormat
    603001.pdf1598KbAdobe PDF2339View/Open


    All items in 政大典藏 are protected by copyright, with all rights reserved.


    社群 sharing

    著作權政策宣告 Copyright Announcement
    1.本網站之數位內容為國立政治大學所收錄之機構典藏,無償提供學術研究與公眾教育等公益性使用,惟仍請適度,合理使用本網站之內容,以尊重著作權人之權益。商業上之利用,則請先取得著作權人之授權。
    The digital content of this website is part of National Chengchi University Institutional Repository. It provides free access to academic research and public education for non-commercial use. Please utilize it in a proper and reasonable manner and respect the rights of copyright owners. For commercial use, please obtain authorization from the copyright owner in advance.

    2.本網站之製作,已盡力防止侵害著作權人之權益,如仍發現本網站之數位內容有侵害著作權人權益情事者,請權利人通知本網站維護人員(nccur@nccu.edu.tw),維護人員將立即採取移除該數位著作等補救措施。
    NCCU Institutional Repository is made to protect the interests of copyright owners. If you believe that any material on the website infringes copyright, please contact our staff(nccur@nccu.edu.tw). We will remove the work from the repository and investigate your claim.
    DSpace Software Copyright © 2002-2004  MIT &  Hewlett-Packard  /   Enhanced by   NTU Library IR team Copyright ©   - Feedback